Acquired hedonic and sensory characteristics of odours: Influence of sweet liker and propylthiouracil taster status
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] J. C. Stevens,et al. PTC taste blindness and the taste of caffeine , 1975, Nature.
[2] D. G. Laing,et al. A quantitative study of fungiform papillae and taste pore density in adults and children. , 2002, Brain research. Developmental brain research.
[3] H. Weingarten,et al. Facial expressions and genetic sensitivity to 6-n-propylthiouracil predict hedonic response to sweet , 1992, Physiology & Behavior.
[4] R. Hancox,et al. Psychosocial correlates of 6-n-propylthiouracil (PROP) ratings in a birth cohort , 2007, Appetite.
[5] Linda M. Bartoshuk,et al. Sweetness of sucrose, neohesperidin dihydrochalcone, and saccharin is related to genetic ability to taste the bitter substance 6-n-propylthiouracil , 1983 .
[6] S. Mobini,et al. Differential hedonic, sensory and behavioral changes associated with flavor–nutrient and flavor–flavor learning , 2008, Physiology & Behavior.
[7] Lawrence E. Marks,et al. Bitterness of KCl and benzoate: Related to genetic status for sensitivity to PTC/PROP. , 1988 .
[8] Judith R Kidd,et al. Supertasting and PROP bitterness depends on more than the TAS2R38 gene. , 2008, Chemical senses.
[9] David J. Mela. Bitter taste intensity: the effect of tastant and thiourea taster status , 1989 .
[10] P. Lovibond,et al. Flavor evaluative conditioning and contingency awareness , 2007, Learning & behavior.
[11] Elizabeth Matisoo-Smith,et al. Taste sensitivity to 6-n-propylthiouracil predicts acceptance of bitter-tasting spinach in 3-6-y-old children. , 2002, The American journal of clinical nutrition.
[12] S. Mobini,et al. Effects of hunger state on flavour pleasantness conditioning at home: Flavour–nutrient learning vs. flavour–flavour learning , 2007, Appetite.
[13] R. Stevenson,et al. Olfactory-induced synesthesias: a review and model. , 2007, Psychological bulletin.
[14] K. Fast. Developing a scale to measure just about anything: comparisons across groups and individuals , 2004 .
[15] B Cowart,et al. Evaluating the 'Labeled Magnitude Scale' for measuring sensations of taste and smell. , 1996, Chemical senses.
[16] P. Schultz,et al. Three sweet receptor genes are clustered in human Chromosome 1 , 2003, Mammalian Genome.
[17] Xiaodong Li,et al. Human receptors for sweet and umami taste , 2002, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[18] S. Mobini,et al. Hunger alters the expression of acquired hedonic but not sensory qualities of food-paired odors in humans. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.
[19] J. Prescott,et al. Responses of PROP taster groups to variations in sensory qualities within foods and beverages , 2004, Physiology & Behavior.
[20] I. Wakeling,et al. Binary taste mixture interactions in prop non-tasters, medium-tasters and super-tasters. , 2001, Chemical senses.
[21] A. Blakeslee. Genetics of Sensory Thresholds: Taste for Phenyl Thio Carbamide. , 1932, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[22] L. Bartoshuk,et al. Bitter taste of saccharin related to the genetic ability to taste the bitter substance 6-n-propylthiouracil. , 1979, Science.
[23] J. Prescott. Comparisons of taste perceptions and preferences of Japanese and Australian consumers: overview and implications for cross-cultural sensory research , 1998 .
[24] V. Duffy,et al. Food acceptance and genetic variation in taste. , 2000, Journal of the American Dietetic Association.
[25] John Prescott,et al. The acquisition of taste properties by odors , 1995 .
[26] I. Martin,et al. Evaluative Conditioning A Case for Hedonic Transfer , 1987 .
[27] A. Blakeslee,et al. Genetics of Sensory Thresholds: Individual Taste Reactions for Different Substances. , 1935, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[28] E. Leach,et al. Comparison of bitterness of caffeine and quinine by a time – intensity procedure , 1986 .
[29] P. Breslin,et al. Genetics of Human Taste Perception on Behalf Of: International and American Associations for Dental Research , 2004 .
[30] R. Boakes,et al. Counter-conditioning Following Human Odor–Taste and Color–Taste Learning ☆ , 2000 .
[31] R. Boakes,et al. Changes in Odor Sweetness Resulting from Implicit Learning of a Simultaneous Odor-Sweetness Association: An Example of Learned Synesthesia ☆ ☆☆ , 1998 .
[32] J. Brunstrom,et al. Flavour–flavour learning occurs automatically and only in hungry participants , 2008, Physiology & Behavior.
[33] Linda M. Bartoshuk,et al. Labeled scales (e.g., category, Likert, VAS) and invalid across-group comparisons: what we have learned from genetic variation in taste , 2003 .
[34] S. Mobini,et al. Hedonic and sensory characteristics of odors conditioned by pairing with tastants in humans. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.
[35] A. Drewnowski,et al. Taste and food preferences as predictors of dietary practices in young women , 1999, Public Health Nutrition.
[36] L. H. Snyder. INHERITED TASTE DEFICIENCY. , 1931, Science.
[37] L. Bartoshuk,et al. Children's food preferences and genetic sensitivity to the bitter taste of 6-n-propylthiouracil (PROP). , 1991, The American journal of clinical nutrition.
[38] S. Mobini,et al. Additive effects of flavour–caffeine and flavour–flavour pairings on liking for the smell and flavour of a novel drink , 2007, Physiology & Behavior.
[39] Frank Baeyens,et al. Flavor-flavor and color-flavor conditioning in humans , 1990 .
[40] Rose Marie Pangborn,et al. Individual variation in affective responses to taste stimuli , 1970 .
[41] V. Duffy,et al. PROP (6‐n‐Propylthiouracil) Supertasters and the Saltiness of NaCl a , 1998, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.
[42] R. Boakes,et al. Resistance to extinction of conditioned odor perceptions: evaluative conditioning is not unique. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[43] A. Drewnowski,et al. Genetic sensitivity to 6-n-propylthiouracil (PROP) and hedonic responses to bitter and sweet tastes. , 1997, Chemical senses.
[44] John Prescott,et al. Human hedonic responses to sweetness: Role of taste genetics and anatomy , 2007, Physiology & Behavior.
[45] Beverly J. Tepper,et al. Development of brief methods to classify individuals by PROP taster status , 2001, Physiology & Behavior.
[46] P. Rozin,et al. Conditioned enhancement of human's liking for flavor by pairing with sweetness. , 1983 .
[47] E. Glanville,et al. Food Preference and Sensitivity of Taste for Bitter Compounds , 1965, Nature.
[48] R. Boakes,et al. Resistance to extinction of conditioned odor perceptions : evaluative conditioning is not unique , 2000 .
[49] J. E. Steiner,et al. Comparative expression of hedonic impact: affective reactions to taste by human infants and other primates , 2001, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.
[50] Linda M. Bartoshuk,et al. PTC/PROP tasting: Anatomy, psychophysics, and sex effects , 1994, Physiology & Behavior.
[51] V. Duffy,et al. Propylthiouracil tasting: determination of underlying threshold distributions using maximum likelihood. , 1995, Chemical senses.
[52] H. Lawless,et al. Bitter taste of saccharin and acesulfame-K. , 2002, Chemical senses.
[53] H. Weingarten,et al. Effects of metabolic state on sweet taste reactivity in humans depend on underlying hedonic response profile , 1991 .
[54] A. Dickinson,et al. Flavor-evaluative conditioning is unaffected by contingency knowledge during training with color-flavor compounds , 2007, Learning & behavior.
[55] P. Rozin,et al. Human food selection: the interaction of biology, culture and individual experience. , 1982 .