Varieties of impulsivity

Abstract The concept of impulsivity covers a wide range of ”actions that are poorly conceived, prematurely expressed, unduly risky, or inappropriate to the situation and that often result in undesirable outcomes”. As such it plays an important role in normal behaviour, as well as, in a pathological form, in many kinds of mental illness such as mania, personality disorders, substance abuse disorders and attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Although evidence from psychological studies of human personality suggests that impulsivity may be made up of several independent factors, this has not made a major impact on biological studies of impulsivity. This may be because there is little unanimity as to which these factors are. The present review summarises evidence for varieties of impulsivity from several different areas of research: human psychology, psychiatry and animal behaviour. Recently, a series of psychopharmacological studies has been carried out by the present author and colleagues using methods proposed to measure selectively different aspects of impulsivity. The results of these studies suggest that several neurochemical mechanisms can influence impulsivity, and that impulsive behaviour has no unique neurobiological basis. Consideration of impulsivity as the result of several different, independent factors which interact to modulate behaviour may provide better insight into the pathology than current hypotheses based on serotonergic underactivity.

[1]  J. Kagan Reflection--impulsivity: the generality and dynamics of conceptual tempo. , 1966, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[2]  R. Herrnstein On the law of effect. , 1970, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[3]  H Rachlin,et al.  Commitment, choice and self-control. , 1972, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[4]  J. Block,et al.  Some Misgivings About the Matching Familiar Figures Test as a Measure of Reflection-Impulsivity. , 1974 .

[5]  B. Srebro,et al.  Behavioral effects of selective midbrain raphe lesions in the rat , 1975, Brain Research.

[6]  C. Carter,et al.  Differential effects of central serotonin manipulation on hyperactive and stereotyped behaviour. , 1978, Life sciences.

[7]  J. E. Mazur,et al.  Choice in a "self-control" paradigm: effects of a fading procedure. , 1978, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[8]  I. Nachshon,et al.  Impulsiveness and aggression among Israeli delinquents. , 1979, The British journal of social and clinical psychology.

[9]  E. Barratt,et al.  The biological basis of impulsiveness: the significance of timing and rhythm disorders , 1983 .

[10]  T. Robbins,et al.  Effects of lesions to ascending noradrenergic neurones on performance of a 5-choice serial reaction task in rats; implications for theories of dorsal noradrenergic bundle function based on selective attention and arousal , 1983, Behavioural Brain Research.

[11]  M Linnoila,et al.  Low cerebrospinal fluid 5-hydroxyindoleacetic acid concentration differentiates impulsive from nonimpulsive violent behavior. , 1983, Life sciences.

[12]  A W Logue,et al.  Choice in a self-control paradigm: Quantification of experience-based differences. , 1984, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[13]  S. Dickman,et al.  Impulsivity and perception: individual differences in the processing of the local and global dimensions of stimuli. , 1985, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[14]  T. Robbins,et al.  Depletion of unilateral striatal dopamine impairs initiation of contralateral actions and not sensory attention , 1985, Nature.

[15]  J. Newman,et al.  Passive avoidance in syndromes of disinhibition: psychopathy and extraversion. , 1985, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[16]  P. Soubrié Reconciling the role of central serotonin neurons in human and animal behavior , 1986, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[17]  C. Bradshaw,et al.  Behaviour of ‘impulsive’ and ‘non-impulsive’ humans in a temporal differentiation schedule of reinforcement , 1987 .

[18]  M. Åsberg,et al.  Markers for vulnerability to psychopathology: Temperament traits associated with platelet MAO activity , 1987, Acta psychiatrica Scandinavica.

[19]  J. Newman,et al.  Reaction to punishment in extraverts and psychopaths: Implications for the impulsive behavior of disinhibited individuals , 1987 .

[20]  C. Robert Cloninger A systematic method for clinical description and classification of personality variants. A proposal. , 1987, Archives of general psychiatry.

[21]  A. Logue Research on self-control: An integrating framework , 1988, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[22]  P. Soubrié,et al.  Effects of imipramine-like drugs and serotonin uptake blockers on delay of reward in rats. Possible implication in the behavioral mechanism of action of antidepressants. , 1988, The Journal of pharmacology and experimental therapeutics.

[23]  T. Robbins,et al.  Effects of unilateral dorsal and ventral striatal dopamine depletion on visual neglect in the rat: A neural and behavioural analysis , 1989, Neuroscience.

[24]  S. Dickman,et al.  PERSONALITY PROCESSES AND INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES Functional and Dysfunctional Impulsivity: Personality and Cognitive Correlates , 1990 .

[25]  C. Bradshaw,et al.  Performance of impulsive and non-impulsive subjects on two temporal differentiation tasks , 1992 .

[26]  S. Dickman,et al.  Impulsivity and information processing. , 1993 .

[27]  C. R. Cloninger,et al.  A psychobiological model of temperament and character. , 1993, Archives of general psychiatry.

[28]  Myrna B. Shure,et al.  The Impulsive client : theory, research, and treatment , 1993 .

[29]  M. Linnoila,et al.  Low CSF 5-HIAA concentrations and severe aggression and impaired impulse control in nonhuman primates. , 1994, The American journal of psychiatry.

[30]  A. Nieoullon,et al.  In a rat model of parkinsonism, lesions of the subthalamic nucleus reverse increases of reaction time but induce a dramatic premature responding deficit , 1995, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[31]  Y. Lecrubier,et al.  The impulsivity rating scale (IRS): preliminary results , 1995, European Psychiatry.

[32]  J. Patton,et al.  Factor structure of the Barratt impulsiveness scale. , 1995, Journal of clinical psychology.

[33]  Howard Rachlin,et al.  Self-control: Beyond commitment. , 1995 .

[34]  P. Svanborg,et al.  Determinants of self‐rating and expert rating concordance in psychiatric out‐patients, using the affective subscales of the CPRS , 1996, Acta psychiatrica Scandinavica.

[35]  P. J. Brasted,et al.  The effect of systemic d-amphetamine on motor versus motivational processes in the rat , 1996, Psychopharmacology.

[36]  P. Jäkälä,et al.  Behavioral and Pharmacological Studies on the Validation of a New Animal Model for Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder , 1996, Neurobiology of Learning and Memory.

[37]  M. Amalric,et al.  Evidence for Functional Differences between Entopeduncular Nucleus and Substantia Nigra: Effects of APV (DL‐2‐amino‐5‐phosphonovaleric acid) Microinfusion on Reaction Time Performance in the Rat , 1996, European Journal of Neuroscience.

[38]  J. Evenden,et al.  The pharmacology of impulsive behaviour in rats: the effects of drugs on response choice with varying delays of reinforcement , 1996, Psychopharmacology.

[39]  R. Hen,et al.  Insights into the Neurobiology of Impulsive Behavior from Serotonin Receptor Knockout Mice , 1997, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[40]  V. Brown,et al.  Deficits in response initiation, but not attention, following excitotoxic lesions of posterior parietal cortex in the rat , 1997, Brain Research.

[41]  Barry J. Everitt,et al.  Central 5-HT depletion enhances impulsive responding without affecting the accuracy of attentional performance: interactions with dopaminergic mechanisms , 1997, Psychopharmacology.

[42]  M. Ho,et al.  5-Hydroxytryptamine and impulse control: prospects for a behavioural analysis , 1998, Journal of psychopharmacology.

[43]  A. Nieoullon,et al.  Impaired performance in a conditioned reaction time task after thermocoagulatory lesions of the fronto-parietal cortex in rats. , 1998, Cerebral cortex.

[44]  J. Evenden The pharmacology of impulsive behaviour in rats III: the effects of amphetamine, haloperidol, imipramine, chlordiazepoxide and ethanol on a paced fixed consecutive number schedule , 1998, Psychopharmacology.

[45]  J. Sirviö,et al.  Changes in activities of dopamine and serotonin systems in the frontal cortex underlie poor choice accuracy and impulsivity of rats in an attention task , 1998, Neuroscience.

[46]  J. Evenden,et al.  The pharmacology of impulsive behaviour in rats VI: the effects of ethanol and selective serotonergic drugs on response choice with varying delays of reinforcement , 1999, Psychopharmacology.

[47]  J. Evenden The pharmacology of impulsive behaviour in rats VII: the effects of serotonergic agonists and antagonists on responding under a discrimination task using unreliable visual stimuli , 1999, Psychopharmacology.

[48]  J. Evenden The pharmacology of impulsive behaviour in rats V: the effects of drugs on responding under a discrimination task using unreliable visual stimuli , 1999, Psychopharmacology.