The effect of perceived speaker age on the perception of PIN and PEN vowels in Houston, Texas

In a study of native listeners' implicit knowledge of the social distribution of a phonological variable we found that the perceived distribution does not completely match the findings of production surveys of the same variable in the same community. This presents an interesting challenge to models of linguistic variation based exclusively on production data, and questions how the distribution of sociolinguistic variables is best defined. The linguistic phenomenon we are investigating is the 'unmerger' of pre-nasal /i/ and /e/ among Anglo Houstonians. This long-standing feature of Southern American English is increasingly losing ground in large metropolitan centers of the South (e.g. Tillery and Bailey 2004). We conducted a speech perception experiment to measure the degree to which Houstonians expect Anglo speakers of three different age groups to participate in the merger. The experiment was conducted using a head-mounted eye-tracker, which monitored the participants' eye movements as they selected on a computer screen the lexical items which they heard (Tanenhaus et al. 2000). Of particular interest were participants' eye fixations on /eN/ and /iN/ competitors, i.e. words which are temporarily ambiguous with the target word in a merged production system. We interpret a greater amount of looks to the competitor as an indicator of the listener's readiness to assume that the speaker may have a merged system. The results show that, as predicted, listeners are more likely to assume a merged system when listening to an "old" speaker than when listening to a "middle-aged" speaker. However, we find no significant difference between the perception of a "middle-aged" and a "young" speaker. These results generally corroborate our recent production surveys in Houston (Gentry 2006; Pantos 2006), which show the merger to be positively correlated with age. However, there is a discrepancy between our participants’ perception and the actual production of Houstonians in the "middle-aged" group. Although, in production terms, native Houstonians in this group pattern with "old" speakers in participating in the merger, listeners expect a "middle-aged" speaker to pattern with a "young" speaker in being less merged. We suggest that this mismatch is due to recent demographic changes in Houston, with large numbers of nonSouthern Anglo speakers moving into the Houston metropolitan area in the course of the Sunbelt migration (Thomas 1997; Klineberg 2006). Native listeners who are exposed to this linguistically mixed population apparently associate merged vowels specifically only with the oldest speakers, even though some younger speakers are also merged. Our study adds to previous research on the perception of vowel merger, including near-merger (Di Paolo and Faber 1990, Labov et al. 1991, inter alia) by providing additional evidence for the role of perceived speaker dialect as a potentially disambiguating factor in speech perception (Hay et al. 2006). Our findings have broad implications for the study of language variation as they challenge sociolinguists to consider whether the variation we seek to describe resides in the “objective” production of speakers native to a community, or rather in the “subjective” experience of its native listeners. This working paper is available in University of Pennsylvania Working Papers in Linguistics: http://repository.upenn.edu/pwpl/ vol14/iss2/12

[1]  Rajend Mesthrie,et al.  A Handbook of Varieties of English , 2004 .

[2]  G. Potts,et al.  Effects of dialect on merger perception: ERP and behavioral correlates , 2005, Brain and Language.

[3]  Elizabeth A. Strand Uncovering the Role of Gender Stereotypes in Speech Perception , 1999 .

[4]  Erik R. Thomas,et al.  A rural/metropolitan split in the speech of Texas Anglos , 1997, Language Variation and Change.

[5]  Robert N. St. Clair,et al.  Language and social psychology , 1981 .

[6]  Elizabeth A. Strand,et al.  Auditory–visual integration of talker gender in vowel perception , 1999 .

[7]  N. Mercer,et al.  Language and Social Psychology , 1979 .

[8]  Nancy Niedzielski,et al.  The Effect of Social Information on the Perception of Sociolinguistic Variables , 1999 .

[9]  B. Kortmann,et al.  Rural Southern white accents , 2008 .

[10]  Natalie Schilling-Estes,et al.  American English: Dialects and Variation , 1998 .

[11]  C. Feagin,et al.  English in the Southern United States: Vowel shifting in the southern states , 2003 .

[12]  W. Labov Principles of Linguistic Change: Internal Factors , 1994 .

[13]  Dennis R. Preston Belle's Body Just Caught the Fit Gnat: The Perception of Northern Cities Shifted Vowels by Local Speakers , 2005 .

[14]  W. Labov,et al.  The Atlas Of North American English , 2005 .

[15]  M K Tanenhaus,et al.  Eye Movements and Lexical Access in Spoken-Language Comprehension: Evaluating a Linking Hypothesis between Fixations and Linguistic Processing , 2000, Journal of psycholinguistic research.

[16]  Jennifer Hay,et al.  Factors influencing speech perception in the context of a merger-in-progress , 2006, J. Phonetics.

[17]  W. Labov Principles Of Linguistic Change , 1994 .

[18]  H. Giles,et al.  Speech style and employment selection: the matched-guise technique , 1982 .

[19]  W. Labov The social stratification of English in New York City , 1969 .

[20]  From bad to bed : The relationship between perceived age and vowel perception in New Zealand English , 2005 .