Action-centred negative priming: Evidence for reactive inhibition

Experiments are described in which the spatial relationship between a stimulus and respondent is held constant in terms of visual and body-centred coordinates, while the complexity of the response is manipulated. It is demonstrated that the degree of complexity of an action directed to the same spatial location determines the level of negative priming observed. This result supports the notions that (1) inhibitory selection mechanisms act on action-centred representations, and (2) the level of inhibition is reactive to the relative potency of the evoked action. The results are also discussed in terms of alternative explanations of negative priming. It is concluded that the results are inconsistent with theories that do not involve inhibitory selection mechanisms.

[1]  S. Tipper,et al.  Selective Reaching to Grasp: Evidence for Distractor Interference Effects , 1997 .

[2]  L Hasher,et al.  On the time course of negative priming: Another look , 1996, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[3]  S. Tipper,et al.  The effects of practice on mechanisms of attention , 1992 .

[4]  E. Fox Negative priming from ignored distractors in visual selection: A review , 1995, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[5]  S. Tipper,et al.  Selective reaching: evidence for action-centered attention. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[6]  D. Lowe Long-term positive and negative identity priming: Evidence for episodic retrieval , 1998, Memory & cognition.

[7]  S P Tipper,et al.  Action-based mechanisms of attention. , 1998, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[8]  R. Ellis,et al.  On the relations between seen objects and components of potential actions. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[9]  F. Lhermitte 'Utilization behaviour' and its relation to lesions of the frontal lobes. , 1983, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[10]  A. Treisman,et al.  Visual memory for novel shapes: implicit coding without attention. , 1996, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[11]  S. Tipper,et al.  Negative priming between pictures and words in a selective attention task: Evidence for semantic processing of ignored stimuli , 1988, Memory & cognition.

[12]  Jeffrey D. Schall,et al.  Neural basis of saccade target selection in frontal eye field during visual search , 1993, Nature.

[13]  N Kanwisher,et al.  Negative priming for spatial locations: identity mismatching, not distractor inhibition. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[14]  M. Posner,et al.  Components of visual orienting , 1984 .

[15]  Bruce Milliken,et al.  Negative priming in a spatial localization task: feature mismatching and distractor inhibition , 1994 .

[16]  A. P. Georgopoulos,et al.  Cortical mechanisms related to the direction of two-dimensional arm movements: relations in parietal area 5 and comparison with motor cortex , 1983, Experimental Brain Research.

[17]  S. Tipper,et al.  Selective reaching: evidence for action-centered attention. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[18]  Cynthia P. May,et al.  Determinants of negative priming. , 1995, Psychological bulletin.

[19]  S. T. Klapp,et al.  REACTION TIME ANALYSIS Of PROGRAMMED CONTROL , 1977, Exercise and sport sciences reviews.

[20]  E. Fox,et al.  Negative priming depends on prime-probe similarity: Evidence for episodic retrieval , 1998 .

[21]  W. Neill Episodic retrieval in negative priming and repetition priming. , 1997 .

[22]  S. Tipper,et al.  Behavioural Goals Determine Inhibitory Mechanisms of Selective Attention , 1994 .

[23]  S. Tipper The Negative Priming Effect: Inhibitory Priming by Ignored Objects , 1985, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[24]  S. Tipper,et al.  Inhibition of return in a selective reaching task: an investigation of reference frames. , 1999, The Journal of general psychology.

[25]  W. Neill,et al.  Persistence of negative priming: II. Evidence for episodic trace retrieval. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[26]  S. Tipper,et al.  A model of inhibitory mechanisms in selective attention. , 1994 .

[27]  S. Tipper,et al.  Selective Attention and Priming: Inhibitory and Facilitatory Effects of Ignored Primes , 1985, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[28]  T. Carr,et al.  Inhibitory Processes in Attention, Memory and Language , 1994 .

[29]  S. Tipper,et al.  Externally cued and internally generated selection: differences in distractor analysis and inhibition. , 1997, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[30]  Stephen Monsell,et al.  The Latency and Duration of Rapid Movement Sequences: Comparisons of Speech and Typewriting , 1978 .

[31]  M. D. Crutcher,et al.  Relations between parameters of step-tracking movements and single cell discharge in the globus pallidus and subthalamic nucleus of the behaving monkey , 1983, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[32]  S P Tipper,et al.  Negative priming between response modalities: Evidence for the central locus of inhibition in selective attention , 1988, Perception & psychophysics.

[33]  F. M. Henry,et al.  Increased Response Latency for Complicated Movements and A “Memory Drum” Theory of Neuromotor Reaction , 1960 .

[34]  S. Tipper,et al.  Reaching into cluttered visual environments: spatial and temporal influences of distracting objects. , 1998, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[35]  S. Tipper,et al.  Reaching into cluttered visual environments: spatial and temporal influences of distracting objects , 1998 .

[36]  S. Tipper,et al.  Inhibition and Interference in Selective Attention: Some Tests of a Neural Network Model , 1996 .

[37]  W. Neill,et al.  Selective attention and the inhibitory control of cognition. , 1995 .

[38]  C. Eriksen,et al.  Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task , 1974 .

[39]  S. Tipper Does Negative Priming Reflect Inhibitory Mechanisms? A Review and Integration of Conflicting Views , 2001, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[40]  W. Neill,et al.  Persistence of negative priming: Steady state or decay? , 1992 .

[41]  R. Desimone,et al.  Selective attention gates visual processing in the extrastriate cortex. , 1985, Science.

[42]  D L Strayer,et al.  Negative priming and perceptual fluency: More than what meets the eye , 2001, Perception & psychophysics.

[43]  S. Tipper,et al.  Selection of moving and static objects for the control of spatially directed action. , 1990, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[44]  D. Kahneman,et al.  The reviewing of object files: Object-specific integration of information , 1992, Cognitive Psychology.

[45]  W. Neill,et al.  Persistence of negative priming: Steady state or decay? , 1992 .

[46]  Andrew R. A. Conway The time-course of negative priming: Little evidence for episodic trace retrieval , 1999, Memory & cognition.

[47]  A E Seiffert,et al.  Selective attention: a reevaluation of the implications of negative priming. , 1998, Psychological review.

[48]  M. Jeannerod,et al.  Optimal response of eye and hand motor systems in pointing at a visual target , 1979, Biological Cybernetics.

[49]  J. Kalaska The representation of arm movements in postcentral and parietal cortex. , 1988, Canadian journal of physiology and pharmacology.

[50]  J. D. Fisk,et al.  The organization of eye and limb movements during unrestricted reaching to targets in contralateral and ipsilateral visual space , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.