Time Is Not Space: Core Computations and Domain-Specific Networks for Mental Travels

Humans can consciously project themselves in the future and imagine themselves at different places. Do mental time travel and mental space navigation abilities share common cognitive and neural mechanisms? To test this, we recorded fMRI while participants mentally projected themselves in time or in space (e.g., 9 years ago, in Paris) and ordered historical events from their mental perspective. Behavioral patterns were comparable for mental time and space and shaped by self-projection and by the distance of historical events to the mental position of the self, suggesting the existence of egocentric mapping in both dimensions. Nonetheless, self-projection in space engaged the medial and lateral parietal cortices, whereas self-projection in time engaged a widespread parietofrontal network. Moreover, while a large distributed network was found for spatial distances, temporal distances specifically engaged the right inferior parietal cortex and the anterior insula. Across these networks, a robust overlap was only found in a small region of the inferior parietal lobe, adding evidence for its role in domain-general egocentric mapping. Our findings suggest that mental travel in time or space capitalizes on egocentric remapping and on distance computation, which are implemented in distinct dimension-specific cortical networks converging in inferior parietal lobe. SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT As humans, we can consciously imagine ourselves at a different time (mental time travel) or at a different place (mental space navigation). Are such abilities domain-general, or are the temporal and spatial dimensions of our conscious experience separable? Here, we tested the hypothesis that mental time travel and mental space navigation required the egocentric remapping of events, including the estimation of their distances to the self. We report that, although both remapping and distance computation are foundational for the processing of the temporal and spatial dimensions of our conscious experience, their neuroanatomical implementations were clearly dissociable and engaged distinct parietal and parietofrontal networks for mental space navigation and mental time travel, respectively.

[1]  A. Craig,et al.  Emotional moments across time: a possible neural basis for time perception in the anterior insula , 2009, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[2]  Olaf Blanke,et al.  Self in Time: Imagined Self-Location Influences Neural Activity Related to Mental Time Travel , 2008, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[3]  Carolyn Parkinson,et al.  A Common Cortical Metric for Spatial, Temporal, and Social Distance , 2014, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[4]  Aiden E. G. F. Arnold,et al.  A critical review of the allocentric spatial representation and its neural underpinnings: toward a network-based perspective , 2014, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[5]  G. Buzsáki,et al.  Memory, navigation and theta rhythm in the hippocampal-entorhinal system , 2013, Nature Neuroscience.

[6]  E. Tulving Episodic memory: from mind to brain. , 2002, Annual review of psychology.

[7]  Vincent Walsh A theory of magnitude: common cortical metrics of time, space and quantity , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[8]  C. Gallistel The organization of learning , 1990 .

[9]  John A. King,et al.  Memory for events and their spatial context: models and experiments. , 2001, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[10]  S. Becker,et al.  Remembering the past and imagining the future: a neural model of spatial memory and imagery. , 2007, Psychological review.

[11]  Alison R. Lane,et al.  Dissociating the neural mechanisms of distance and spatial reference frames , 2015, Neuropsychologia.

[12]  Christian F. Doeller,et al.  Imagining being somewhere else: neural basis of changing perspective in space. , 2012, Cerebral cortex.

[13]  Katherine R. Sherrill,et al.  Hippocampus and Retrosplenial Cortex Combine Path Integration Signals for Successful Navigation , 2013, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[14]  A. Berthoz,et al.  Reference Frames for Spatial Cognition: Different Brain Areas are Involved in Viewer-, Object-, and Landmark-Centered Judgments About Object Location , 2004, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[15]  Margot J. Taylor,et al.  Is 2+2=4? Meta-analyses of brain areas needed for numbers and calculations , 2011, NeuroImage.

[16]  Katiuscia Sacco,et al.  Functional connectivity of the insula in the resting brain , 2011, NeuroImage.

[17]  A. Berthoz,et al.  Mental navigation along memorized routes activates the hippocampus, precuneus, and insula , 1997, Neuroreport.

[18]  Jesper Andersson,et al.  Valid conjunction inference with the minimum statistic , 2005, NeuroImage.

[19]  Olaf Blanke,et al.  Subjective mental time: the functional architecture of projecting the self to past and future , 2009, The European journal of neuroscience.

[20]  Baptiste Gauthier,et al.  Cognitive mapping in mental time travel and mental space navigation , 2016, Cognition.

[21]  Marisa O. Hollinshead,et al.  The organization of the human cerebral cortex estimated by intrinsic functional connectivity. , 2011, Journal of neurophysiology.

[22]  G. Lakoff,et al.  Metaphors We Live By , 1980 .

[23]  Benjamin J. Shannon,et al.  Parietal lobe contributions to episodic memory retrieval , 2005, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[24]  R. Shepard,et al.  Mental Rotation of Three-Dimensional Objects , 1971, Science.

[25]  Alain Berthoz,et al.  Role of the human retrosplenial cortex/parieto-occipital sulcus in perspective priming , 2016, NeuroImage.

[26]  Donna Rose Addis,et al.  Mental time travel and the shaping of the human mind , 2009, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[27]  Christian F. Doeller,et al.  Anterior Hippocampus and Goal-Directed Spatial Decision Making , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[28]  E. Tulving,et al.  Consciousness of subjective time in the brain , 2010, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[29]  Steen Moeller,et al.  Evaluation of slice accelerations using multiband echo planar imaging at 3T , 2013, NeuroImage.

[30]  P. Cavanagh,et al.  Opinion TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences Vol.11 No.5 The ‘when ’ pathway of the right parietal lobe , 2022 .

[31]  Arne D. Ekstrom,et al.  Multiple interacting brain areas underlie successful spatiotemporal memory retrieval in humans , 2014, Scientific Reports.

[32]  M. Zorzi,et al.  When time is space: Evidence for a mental time line , 2012, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[33]  R. Baayen,et al.  Mixed-effects modeling with crossed random effects for subjects and items , 2008 .

[34]  Pascale Piolino,et al.  Mental time travel into the past and the future in healthy aged adults: An fMRI study , 2011, Brain and Cognition.

[35]  Elizabeth M. Brannon,et al.  Space, time, and number: a Kantian research program , 2010, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[36]  Russell A. Epstein,et al.  Anchoring the neural compass: Coding of local spatial reference frames in human medial parietal lobe , 2014, Nature Neuroscience.

[37]  Angela D Friederici,et al.  The language network , 2012, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[38]  M. D’Esposito,et al.  Topographical disorientation: a synthesis and taxonomy. , 1999, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[39]  A. Cowey,et al.  Near space functioning of the human angular and supramarginal gyri. , 2009, Journal of neuropsychology.

[40]  Neil Burgess,et al.  Examining the role of the temporo-parietal network in memory, imagery, and viewpoint transformations , 2014, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[41]  Yaacov Trope,et al.  Traversing psychological distance , 2014, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[42]  Andrew W. Young,et al.  Egocentric Disorientation following Bilateral Parietal Lobe Damage , 2005, Cortex.

[43]  Arne D. Ekstrom,et al.  Differential Recruitment of Brain Networks following Route and Cartographic Map Learning of Spatial Environments , 2012, PloS one.

[44]  E. Maguire,et al.  A Temporoparietal and Prefrontal Network for Retrieving the Spatial Context of Lifelike Events , 2001, NeuroImage.

[45]  Roberto Cabeza,et al.  Role of parietal regions in episodic memory retrieval: The dual attentional processes hypothesis , 2008, Neuropsychologia.

[46]  Y. Trope,et al.  The Psychology of Transcending the Here and Now , 2008, Science.

[47]  Olaf Blanke,et al.  Brain system for mental orientation in space, time, and person , 2015, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[48]  Russell A. Epstein Parahippocampal and retrosplenial contributions to human spatial navigation , 2008, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[49]  R. Moyer,et al.  Mental comparison and the symbolic distance effect , 1976, Cognitive Psychology.

[50]  G. Orban,et al.  Processing of Abstract Ordinal Knowledge in the Horizontal Segment of the Intraparietal Sulcus , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[51]  Morris Moscovitch,et al.  Mental space travel: damage to posterior parietal cortex prevents egocentric navigation and reexperiencing of remote spatial memories. , 2010, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[52]  E. Denkova,et al.  Experiencing past and future personal events: Functional neuroimaging evidence on the neural bases of mental time travel , 2008, Brain and Cognition.

[53]  S M Kosslyn,et al.  Visual images preserve metric spatial information: evidence from studies of image scanning. , 1978, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[54]  L. Boroditsky Metaphoric structuring: understanding time through spatial metaphors , 2000, Cognition.

[55]  S. Dehaene,et al.  A Magnitude Code Common to Numerosities and Number Symbols in Human Intraparietal Cortex , 2007, Neuron.

[56]  D. Bates,et al.  Fitting Linear Mixed-Effects Models Using lme4 , 2014, 1406.5823.

[57]  Kaustubh Supekar,et al.  Dissociable connectivity within human angular gyrus and intraparietal sulcus: evidence from functional and structural connectivity. , 2010, Cerebral cortex.

[58]  Olaf Blanke,et al.  The mental time line: An analogue of the mental number line in the mapping of life events , 2009, Consciousness and Cognition.