Cooperation and acting for the greater good during the COVID-19 pandemic
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] Rolf A. Zwaan,et al. National identity predicts public health support during a global pandemic , 2022, Nature Communications.
[2] C. Betsch,et al. Reactance revisited: Consequences of mandatory and scarce vaccination in the case of COVID‐19 , 2021, Applied psychology. Health and well-being.
[3] Jackson G. Lu,et al. Collectivism predicts mask use during COVID-19 , 2021, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[4] S. Lewandowsky,et al. Effects of different types of written vaccination information on COVID-19 vaccine hesitancy in the UK (OCEANS-III): a single-blind, parallel-group, randomised controlled trial , 2021, The Lancet Public Health.
[5] Jason M Etchegaray,et al. Trust in experts, not trust in national leadership, leads to greater uptake of recommended actions during the COVID‐19 pandemic , 2021, Risk, hazards & crisis in public policy.
[6] L. Macchi,et al. Behavioral Changes After the COVID-19 Lockdown in Italy , 2021, Frontiers in Psychology.
[7] A. Milstein,et al. Influence of a COVID-19 vaccine’s effectiveness and safety profile on vaccination acceptance , 2021, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[8] I. E. Gozum. Common good and public service as vital components for government officials in promoting COVID-19 vaccination , 2021, Journal of public health.
[9] J. Brooks,et al. Effectiveness of Mask Wearing to Control Community Spread of SARS-CoV-2. , 2021, JAMA.
[10] F. Alla,et al. COVID-19 vaccine hesitancy in a representative working-age population in France: a survey experiment based on vaccine characteristics , 2021, The Lancet Public Health.
[11] H. Larson,et al. Measuring the impact of COVID-19 vaccine misinformation on vaccination intent in the UK and USA , 2021, Nature Human Behaviour.
[12] V. Capraro,et al. Telling people to “rely on their reasoning” increases intentions to wear a face covering to slow down COVID‐19 transmission , 2021, Applied cognitive psychology.
[13] Robert West,et al. The effect of spokesperson attribution on public health message sharing during the COVID-19 pandemic , 2021, PloS one.
[14] Valerio Capraro,et al. Mathematical foundations of moral preferences , 2021, Journal of the Royal Society Interface.
[15] D. Albarracín,et al. Concerns for others increases the likelihood of vaccination against influenza and COVID-19 more in sparsely rather than densely populated areas , 2020, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[16] E. Cvejic,et al. Concerns and motivations about COVID-19 vaccination , 2020, The Lancet Infectious Diseases.
[17] M. Paulus,et al. Prevention for oneself or others? Psychological and social factors that explain social distancing during the COVID-19 pandemic , 2020, Journal of health psychology.
[18] Feng Fu,et al. Oscillatory dynamics in the dilemma of social distancing , 2020, Proceedings of the Royal Society A.
[19] T. Campbell,et al. Barriers and facilitators of adherence to social distancing recommendations during COVID-19 among a large international sample of adults , 2020, PloS one.
[20] M. Petersen,et al. Information about herd immunity through vaccination and empathy promote COVID-19 vaccination intentions. , 2020, Health psychology : official journal of the Division of Health Psychology, American Psychological Association.
[21] A. Murray,et al. Non-compliance with COVID-19-related public health measures among young adults in Switzerland: Insights from a longitudinal cohort study , 2020, Social Science & Medicine.
[22] H. Mercier,et al. Improving preventive health behaviors in the COVID-19 crisis: a messaging intervention in a large nationally representative sample , 2020 .
[23] S. Banker,et al. Evaluating Prosocial COVID-19 Messaging Frames: Evidence from a Field Study on Facebook , 2020, SSRN Electronic Journal.
[24] A. Vespignani,et al. Modelling the impact of testing, contact tracing and household quarantine on second waves of COVID-19 , 2020, Nature Human Behaviour.
[25] Lars Korn,et al. Vaccination as a social contract , 2020, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[26] T. Mitze,et al. Face masks considerably reduce COVID-19 cases in Germany , 2020, medRxiv.
[27] V. Latora,et al. Networks beyond pairwise interactions: structure and dynamics , 2020, Physics Reports.
[28] C. Bauch,et al. Prosocial polio vaccination in Israel , 2020, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[29] M. J. Pedersen,et al. How to encourage “Togetherness by Keeping Apart” amid COVID-19? The ineffectiveness of prosocial and empathy appeals , 2020 .
[30] Armando N. Meier,et al. Prosociality predicts health behaviors during the COVID-19 pandemic , 2020, Journal of Public Economics.
[31] J. Savoie,et al. Does Collective Interest or Self-Interest Motivate Mask Usage as a Preventive Measure Against COVID-19? , 2020, Canadian Journal of Political Science.
[32] J. Zaki,et al. Catastrophe Compassion: Understanding and Extending Prosociality Under Crisis , 2020, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[33] V. Capraro,et al. The effect of messaging and gender on intentions to wear a face covering to slow down COVID-19 transmission , 2020, 2005.05467.
[34] V. Capraro,et al. The effect of norm-based messages on reading and understanding COVID-19 pandemic response governmental rules , 2020, 2005.03998.
[35] A. Deslatte. To shop or shelter? Issue framing effects and social-distancing preferences in the COVID-19 pandemic , 2020 .
[36] W. Hippel,et al. Helping behavior is non-zero-sum: Helper and recipient autobiographical accounts of help , 2020 .
[37] Marc-Lluís Vives,et al. Emotional responses to prosocial messages increase willingness to self-isolate during the COVID-19 pandemic , 2020, Personality and Individual Differences.
[38] David G. Rand,et al. Using social and behavioural science to support COVID-19 pandemic response , 2020, Nature Human Behaviour.
[39] Jessica T Davis,et al. The effect of travel restrictions on the spread of the 2019 novel coronavirus (COVID-19) outbreak , 2020, Science.
[40] H. Julienne,et al. Motivating social distancing during the COVID-19 pandemic: An online experiment. , 2020, Social science & medicine.
[41] Vito Latora,et al. Evolutionary dynamics of higher-order interactions in social networks , 2020, Nature Human Behaviour.
[42] Andrew J. Vonasch,et al. Future-Orientation (as Trait and State) Promotes Reputation-Protective Choice in Moral Dilemmas , 2019, Social Psychological and Personality Science.
[43] Valerio Capraro,et al. The Dual-Process Approach to Human Sociality: A Review , 2019, SSRN Electronic Journal.
[44] Patrick Vinck,et al. Institutional trust and misinformation in the response to the 2018-19 Ebola outbreak in North Kivu, DR Congo: a population-based survey. , 2019, The Lancet. Infectious diseases.
[45] M. Johannesson,et al. The intuitive cooperation hypothesis revisited: a meta-analytic examination of effect size and between-study heterogeneity , 2019, Journal of the Economic Science Association.
[46] David G. Rand,et al. Signaling when no one is watching: A reputation heuristics account of outrage and punishment in one-shot anonymous interactions. , 2019, Journal of personality and social psychology.
[47] Martin A. Nowak,et al. Evolution of cooperation on large networks with community structure , 2019, Journal of the Royal Society Interface.
[48] C. Betsch,et al. Beyond confidence: Development of a measure assessing the 5C psychological antecedents of vaccination , 2018, PloS one.
[49] Yi Lei,et al. Commentary: Are groups more or less than the sum of their members? The moderating role of individual identification , 2018, Front. Psychol..
[50] Martin A. Nowak,et al. Conjoining uncooperative societies facilitates evolution of cooperation , 2018, Nature Human Behaviour.
[51] David G. Rand,et al. Do the Right Thing: Experimental Evidence that Preferences for Moral Behavior, Rather Than Equity or Efficiency per se, Drive Human Prosociality , 2018, Judgment and Decision Making.
[52] Mathijs Mul,et al. Increasing altruistic and cooperative behaviour with simple moral nudges , 2017, Scientific Reports.
[53] V. Latora,et al. Complex Networks: Principles, Methods and Applications , 2017 .
[54] H. Sjåstad,et al. Short-sighted Greed? Focusing on the Future Promotes Reputation-based Generosity (trans. Maria Yu. Beletskaya) , 2017, Scientific Research of Faculty of Economics. Electronic Journal.
[55] Xingru Chen,et al. Leveraging statistical physics to improve understanding of cooperation in multiplex networks , 2017, New journal of physics.
[56] Attila Szolnoki,et al. Statistical Physics of Human Cooperation , 2017, ArXiv.
[57] Matjaz Perc,et al. Determinants of public cooperation in multiplex networks , 2017, ArXiv.
[58] David G. Rand,et al. Signaling Emotion and Reason in Cooperation , 2017, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[59] Dawei Zhao,et al. Statistical physics of vaccination , 2016, ArXiv.
[60] Cornelia Betsch,et al. Detrimental effects of introducing partial compulsory vaccination: experimental evidence. , 2016, European journal of public health.
[61] Matjaz Perc,et al. Evolutionary mixed games in structured populations: Cooperation and the benefits of heterogeneity , 2016, Physical review. E.
[62] Cornelia Betsch,et al. Using Behavioral Insights to Increase Vaccination Policy Effectiveness , 2015 .
[63] Attila Szolnoki,et al. Stability of cooperation under image scoring in group interactions , 2015, Scientific Reports.
[64] Arianna Novaro,et al. Does Telling White Lies Signal Pro-Social Preferences? , 2015, Judgment and Decision Making.
[65] C. Engel,et al. Conditional Cooperation With Negative Externalities – An Experiment , 2014 .
[66] David G. Rand,et al. Reflection does not undermine self-interested prosociality , 2014, Front. Behav. Neurosci..
[67] David G. Rand,et al. Heuristics guide the implementation of social preferences in one-shot Prisoner's Dilemma experiments , 2014, Scientific Reports.
[68] David G. Rand,et al. Social heuristics shape intuitive cooperation , 2014, Nature Communications.
[69] Attila Szolnoki,et al. Evolutionary dynamics of group interactions on structured populations: a review , 2013, Journal of The Royal Society Interface.
[70] Luis Mario Floría,et al. Evolution of Cooperation in Multiplex Networks , 2012, Scientific Reports.
[71] F. C. Santos,et al. The role of diversity in the evolution of cooperation. , 2012, Journal of theoretical biology.
[72] M. Archetti,et al. Review: Game theory of public goods in one-shot social dilemmas without assortment. , 2012, Journal of theoretical biology.
[73] Attila Szolnoki,et al. Evolution of public cooperation on interdependent networks: The impact of biased utility functions , 2012, ArXiv.
[74] Susan K. Walker. Connected: The Surprising Power of Our Social Networks and How They Shape Our Lives , 2011 .
[75] Jari Saramäki,et al. Temporal Networks , 2011, Encyclopedia of Social Network Analysis and Mining.
[76] P. V. van Lange,et al. How the shadow of the future might promote cooperation , 2011 .
[77] Florian Wakolbinger,et al. To lead or not to lead: Endogenous sequencing in public goods games , 2010 .
[78] Attila Szolnoki,et al. Coevolutionary Games - A Mini Review , 2009, Biosyst..
[79] Attila Szolnoki,et al. Topology-independent impact of noise on cooperation in spatial public goods games. , 2009, Physical review. E, Statistical, nonlinear, and soft matter physics.
[80] Martin A Nowak,et al. Evolutionary dynamics in set structured populations , 2009, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[81] G. Camera,et al. Cooperation among Strangers under the Shadow of the Future , 2009 .
[82] F. C. Santos,et al. Social diversity promotes the emergence of cooperation in public goods games , 2008, Nature.
[83] M. Perc,et al. Social diversity and promotion of cooperation in the spatial prisoner's dilemma game. , 2007, Physical review. E, Statistical, nonlinear, and soft matter physics.
[84] J. Henrich,et al. Why Humans Cooperate: A Cultural and Evolutionary Explanation , 2007 .
[85] Francisco C. Santos,et al. Cooperation Prevails When Individuals Adjust Their Social Ties , 2006, PLoS Comput. Biol..
[86] Robert Leonard. Theory of Games and Economic Behavior , 2006 .
[87] F. C. Santos,et al. Scale-free networks provide a unifying framework for the emergence of cooperation. , 2005, Physical review letters.
[88] M. Milinski,et al. Cooperation through indirect reciprocity: image scoring or standing strategy? , 2001, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.
[89] S. Gächter,et al. Reputation and Reciprocity: Consequences for the Labour Relation , 2001, SSRN Electronic Journal.
[90] Erling Moxnes,et al. The Effect of Leadership in a Public Bad Experiment , 2000 .
[91] M. Nowak,et al. Evolution of indirect reciprocity by image scoring , 1998, Nature.
[92] M. Nowak,et al. Evolutionary games and spatial chaos , 1992, Nature.
[93] P. Richerson,et al. The evolution of indirect reciprocity , 1989 .
[94] John Orbell,et al. Not me or thee but we: the importance of group identity in eliciting cooperation in dilemma situatio , 1988 .
[95] Robert M. May,et al. The evolution of cooperation , 1981, Science.
[96] R. Trivers. The Evolution of Reciprocal Altruism , 1971, The Quarterly Review of Biology.
[97] W. Hamilton. The genetical evolution of social behaviour. II. , 1964, Journal of theoretical biology.
[98] A. Gouldner. THE NORM OF RECIPROCITY: A PRELIMINARY STATEMENT * , 1960 .
[99] Lily L. Tsai,et al. Public health and public trust: Survey evidence from the Ebola Virus Disease epidemic in Liberia. , 2017, Social science & medicine.
[100] Erik O. Kimbrough,et al. Norms Make Preferences Social ⇤ , 2015 .
[101] Astrid Dannenberg,et al. Leading by example versus leading by words in voluntary contribution experiments , 2015, Soc. Choice Welf..
[102] Pat Barclay,et al. A cue of kinship promotes cooperation for the public good , 2008 .
[103] BÓ Pedrodal,et al. Cooperation under the Shadow of the Future : Experimental Evidence from Infinitely Repeated Games , 2005 .
[104] C. Berge. Hypergraphs: Combinatorics of Finite Sets , 1989 .