Delayed–Execute Prospective Memory Performance: The Effects of Age and Working Memory

This study follows the novel delayed-execute prospective memory paradigm, which involves briefly delaying the execution of an intended action, a task that has been shown to produce substantial age effects. During the ongoing task, sentences were presented, and participants had to answer reading-comprehension questions and general knowledge questions. In the prospective memory task, the participant was to press a key after the presentation of a specific cue in the sentences—but not before a subsequent phase of the ongoing task was reached. In contrast to previous studies using older participants taken from very broadly defined age ranges, this study examines development of delayed-execute prospective memory more precisely by examining a total of 4 age groups: a younger age group (age range = 22–31; n = 27), a young-old age group (age range = 60–69; n = 34), a middle-old age group (age range = 70–79; n = 31), and an old-old age group (age range = 80–91; n = 35). This study investigates the dependence of (age-related) delayed-execute prospective memory performance on working memory capacity by disrupting the phonological loop during the delay period as well as its dependence on neuropsychological processes such as inhibitory control and processing speed. The results show that (a) delayed-execute prospective memory particularly declines within the group of older participants, (b) delayed-execute prospective memory is diminished when working memory load is high during the delay period, and (c) age-related performance in delayed-execute prospective memory may be mediated by inhibitory control. The findings are discussed in the context of the frontal lobe hypothesis of cognitive aging.

[1]  R. Kawashima,et al.  Participation of the prefrontal cortices in prospective memory: evidence from a PET study in humans , 1998, Neuroscience Letters.

[2]  R. Martin,et al.  Prospective remembering in younger and older adults: Role of the prospective cue , 2001, Memory.

[3]  E. Maylor,et al.  Age-related impairment in an event-based prospective-memory task. , 1996, Psychology and aging.

[4]  J. Ellis,et al.  Prospective Memory in 2000: Past, Present, and Future Directions , 2000 .

[5]  M. McDaniel,et al.  Age-related deficits in prospective memory: the influence of task complexity. , 1992, Psychology and aging.

[6]  G L Allen,et al.  Aging, cognitive resources, and declarative learning. , 1996, Psychology and aging.

[7]  S. L. Richardson,et al.  Aging and prospective memory: examining the influences of self-initiated retrieval processes. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[8]  C B Mayhorn,et al.  Effect of age on event-based and time-based prospective memory. , 1997, Psychology and aging.

[9]  D. Wechsler Manual for the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale. , 1955 .

[10]  R. Marsh,et al.  The demands of an ongoing activity influence the success of event-based prospective memory , 2002, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[11]  K. Kerns,et al.  An Investigation of Prospective Memory in Children With ADHD , 2001, Child neuropsychology : a journal on normal and abnormal development in childhood and adolescence.

[12]  Sergio Della Sala,et al.  Working memory in event‐ and time‐based prospective memory tasks: Effects of secondary demand and age , 2004 .

[13]  F. Craik,et al.  Influences on the efficiency of prospective memory in younger and older adults. , 2001, Psychology and aging.

[14]  Karen L. Siedlecki,et al.  Construct validity and age sensitivity of prospective memory , 2004, Memory & cognition.

[15]  J. Cockburn,et al.  Task Interruption in Prospective Memory: A Frontal Lobe Function? , 1995, Cortex.

[16]  C. Braun,et al.  Cognitive Structure of Executive Deficits in Frontally Lesioned Head Trauma Patients Performing Activities of Daily Living , 2003, Cortex.

[17]  A. Baddeley,et al.  Working Memory and Vigilance: Evidence from Normal Aging and Alzheimer's Disease , 1999, Brain and Cognition.

[18]  T. Cutmore,et al.  Performance of individuals with severe long-term traumatic brain injury on time-, event-, and activity-based prospective memory tasks. , 1999, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[19]  T. Salthouse The processing-speed theory of adult age differences in cognition. , 1996, Psychological review.

[20]  K. Cherry,et al.  Age and individual differences influence prospective memory. , 1999, Psychology and aging.

[21]  C. Hall,et al.  Age, gesture span, and dissociations among component subsystems of working memory. , 2000, Brain and cognition.

[22]  R. Marsh,et al.  Event-based prospective memory and executive control of working memory. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[23]  Roger W. Morrell,et al.  Prospective memory and aging: The effects of working memory and prospective memory task load , 1997 .

[24]  Felicia A. Huppert,et al.  High prevalence of prospective memory impairment in the elderly and in early-stage dementia: Findings from a population-based study , 2000 .

[25]  M. McDaniel,et al.  The involvement of executive functions in prospective memory performance of adults , 2003 .

[26]  M. McDaniel,et al.  Forgetting of intentions in demanding situations is rapid. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Applied.

[27]  J. Jolles,et al.  Stroop interference: aging effects assessed with the Stroop Color-Word Test. , 1993, Experimental aging research.

[28]  Roger Remington,et al.  Prospective memory in dynamic environments: Effects of load, delay, and phonological rehearsal , 2001, Memory.

[29]  M. D’Esposito Working memory. , 2008, Handbook of clinical neurology.

[30]  R. West,et al.  Effects of aging on event-related neural activity related to prospective memory , 2001, Neuroreport.

[31]  F. Huppert,et al.  Prospective memory impairment as an early indicator of dementia. , 1993, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[32]  Timothy A. Salthouse,et al.  The aging of working memory. , 1994 .

[33]  Iroise Dumontheil,et al.  On the role of rostral prefrontal cortex (area 10) in prospective memory , 2008 .

[34]  Melissa J. Guynn,et al.  Prospective memory: a neuropsychological study. , 1999, Neuropsychology.

[35]  T. Salthouse,et al.  Decomposing adult age differences in working memory. , 1991 .

[36]  Peter Graf,et al.  Pro- and Retrospective Memory in Late Adulthood , 2001, Consciousness and Cognition.

[37]  S. Scott,et al.  The role of the rostral frontal cortex (area 10) in prospective memory: a lateral versus medial dissociation , 2003, Neuropsychologia.

[38]  M. Kliegel,et al.  Prospective memory research: Why is it relevant? , 2003 .

[39]  J. Henry,et al.  A meta-analytic review of prospective memory and aging. , 2004, Psychology and aging.

[40]  G. Scerif,et al.  Executive Functioning as a Predictor of Children's Mathematics Ability: Inhibition, Switching, and Working Memory , 2001, Developmental neuropsychology.

[41]  T. Shallice,et al.  The cognitive and neuroanatomical correlates of multitasking , 2000, Neuropsychologia.

[42]  Elizabeth A. Maylor,et al.  Changes in Event-Based Prospective Memory Across Adulthood , 1998 .

[43]  P. Warr,et al.  Age and working memory: the role of perceptual speed, the central executive, and the phonological loop. , 1996, Psychology and aging.

[44]  M. McDaniel,et al.  (www.interscience.wiley.com) DOI: 10.1002/acp.1002 Delaying Execution of Intentions: Overcoming the Costs of Interruptions , 2022 .

[45]  E A Maylor Aging and forgetting in prospective and retrospective memory tasks. , 1993, Psychology and aging.

[46]  J. Ridley Studies of Interference in Serial Verbal Reactions , 2001 .

[47]  M. McDaniel,et al.  Prospective memory and aging: forgetting intentions over short delays. , 2000, Psychology and aging.

[48]  D. Balota,et al.  Stroop performance in healthy younger and older adults and in individuals with dementia of the Alzheimer's type. , 1996, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[49]  T. Mäntylä,et al.  Remembering to remember in adulthood: A population-based study on aging and prospective memory , 1997 .

[50]  A D Baddeley,et al.  The decline of working memory in Alzheimer's disease. A longitudinal study. , 1991, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[51]  M. McDaniel,et al.  Normal aging and prospective memory. , 1990, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[52]  R. West,et al.  An application of prefrontal cortex function theory to cognitive aging. , 1996, Psychological bulletin.

[53]  C. Frith,et al.  Brain regions involved in prospective memory as determined by positron emission tomography , 2001, Neuropsychologia.

[54]  Matthias Kliegel,et al.  Planning and realization of complex intentions in traumatic brain injury and normal aging , 2004, Brain and Cognition.

[55]  Mark A McDaniel,et al.  Aging and maintaining intentions over delays: do it or lose it. , 2003, Psychology and aging.