"Warmth-insensitive fields": evidence of sparse and irregular innervation of human skin by the warmth sense.

Although more acute in some areas of the body than in others, temperature sensitivity is assumed to be present throughout the skin. Only when very small stimuli have been used (e.g., approximately 1 mm2) has sensitivity to warming or cooling appeared discontinuous. Here we report the discovery of patches of skin several square centimeters in area within which heating cannot be detected until skin temperature exceeds the thresholds of C heat-sensitive nociceptors (>41 degrees C). These warmth-insensitive fields (> or = 5 cm2), which appear to lack low-threshold warm fibers, were also found to have reduced responsiveness to non-painful heating and significantly higher heat pain thresholds compared to surrounding areas of skin. The existence of such sites corroborates reports that warm fibers are rare in human cutaneous nerves and confirms the classical theory that cutaneous innervation by the warmth sense is punctate and sparse. The insensitive areas also provide unique opportunities for assessing the contribution of the low-threshold warmth system to perception of heat and heat pain, and their existence in healthy young adults contraindicates use of warmth sensitivity in neurological assessments of C-fiber function.

[1]  R. LaMotte,et al.  Peripheral neural correlates of magnitude of cutaneous pain and hyperalgesia: simultaneous recordings in humans of sensory judgments of pain and evoked responses in nociceptors with C-fibers. , 1984, Journal of neurophysiology.

[2]  J. Greenspan,et al.  The primate as a model for the human temperature-sensing system: 2. Area of skin receiving thermal stimulation (spatial summation). , 1985, Somatosensory research.

[3]  J. Mchaffie,et al.  Spatial summation of heat-induced pain: influence of stimulus area and spatial separation of stimuli on perceived pain sensation intensity and unpleasantness. , 1989, Journal of neurophysiology.

[4]  L. Watkins,et al.  Spatial summation in human thermal pain perception: comparison within and between dermatomes , 1992, Pain.

[5]  J D Hardy,et al.  STUDIES IN TEMPERATURE SENSATION. III. THE SENSITIVITY OF THE BODY TO HEAT AND THE SPATIAL SUMMATION OF THE END ORGAN RESPONSES. , 1937, The Journal of clinical investigation.

[6]  H. Wolff,et al.  STUDIES ON PAIN. A NEW METHOD FOR MEASURING PAIN THRESHOLD: OBSERVATIONS ON SPATIAL SUMMATION OF PAIN , 1940, The Journal of clinical investigation.

[7]  H. E. Torebjörk,et al.  Nociceptors and warm receptors innervated by C fibres in human skin. , 1982, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[8]  J. Gybels,et al.  A comparison between the discharges of human nociceptive nerve fibres and the subject's ratings of his sensations. , 1979 .

[9]  B. Green,et al.  Measuring the Chemosensory Irritability of Human Skin , 1995 .

[10]  K. O. Johnson,et al.  Peripheral neural determinants of temperature discrimination in man: a correlative study of responses to cooling skin. , 1973, Journal of neurophysiology.

[11]  L E Marks,et al.  Regional sensitivity and spatial summation in the warmth sense. , 1974, Physiology & behavior.

[12]  W S Cain,et al.  Spatial discrimination of cutaneous warmth. , 1973, The American journal of psychology.

[13]  W. L. Jenkins Studies in thermal sensitivity: 16. Further evidence on the effects of stimulus temperature. , 1941 .

[14]  J. Ochoa,et al.  Warm and cold specific somatosensory systems. Psychophysical thresholds, reaction times and peripheral conduction velocities. , 1991, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[15]  A. Pertovaara,et al.  The effects of stimulus area and adaptation temperature on warm and heat pain thresholds in man. , 1987, The International journal of neuroscience.

[16]  The Temperature Spots and End-Organs , 1927 .

[17]  R. Dykes,et al.  "Cold" fiber population innervating palmar and digital skin of the monkey: responses to cooling pulses. , 1973, Journal of neurophysiology.

[18]  J. Ochoa,et al.  Quantitative somatosensory thermotest. A key method for functional evaluation of small calibre afferent channels. , 1992, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[19]  S. Chéry-croze,et al.  Spatial summation of thermal pain in human beings. , 1979, Sensory processes.

[20]  Spatial summation of pain. , 1958, Journal of applied physiology.

[21]  P C O'Brien,et al.  Cool, warm, and heat‐pain detection thresholds , 1993, Neurology.

[22]  K. O. Johnson,et al.  Warm fibers innervating palmar and digital skin of the monkey: responses to thermal stimuli. , 1979, Journal of neurophysiology.

[23]  L E Marks,et al.  Interaction of spatial and temporal summation in the warmth sense. , 1976, Sensory processes.

[24]  B. Green,et al.  Localization of thermal sensation: An illusion and synthetic heat , 1977 .

[25]  G. B. Wetherill,et al.  SEQUENTIAL ESTIMATION OF POINTS ON A PSYCHOMETRIC FUNCTION. , 1965, The British journal of mathematical and statistical psychology.

[26]  D. Yarnitsky,et al.  Single C nociceptor responses and psychophysical parameters of evoked pain: effect of rate of rise of heat stimuli in humans. , 1992, The Journal of physiology.

[27]  J Gybels,et al.  C nociceptor activity in human nerve during painful and non painful skin stimulation. , 1981, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[28]  Lawrence E. Marks,et al.  Spatial summation and the dynamics of warmth sensation , 1971 .

[29]  R MELZACK,et al.  Skin sensitivity to thermal stimuli. , 1962, Experimental neurology.

[30]  E. Dodt,et al.  Mode of action of warm receptors. , 1952, Acta physiologica Scandinavica.

[31]  J D Greenspan,et al.  Somatotopic localization of thermal stimuli: I. A comparison of within- versus across-dermatomal separation of innocuous thermal stimuli. , 1996, Somatosensory & motor research.