Examples, rules, and strategies in the control of dynamic systems.

Two main types of knowledge are considered relevant to successful control of dynamic systems: input-output knowledge (I-O-knowledge), which represents specific input values together with the corresponding output values, and structural knowledge, defined as general knowledge about the variables of a system and their causal relations. While I-O-knowledge has proven important for the control of small systems, structural knowledge is expected to enhance performance when dealing with more complex systems. In an experiment, structural knowledge about a complex system was manipulated. Although the experimental group had better structural knowledge, the control group was equally successful in reaching new goals. That seems to contradict other studies where effects of structural knowledge on performance have been found. To resolve these contradictions, the consideration of a third type of knowledge strategic knowledge is suggested. The postulated effects of different levels of structural and strategic knowledge are explored with a computational model. The three knowledge types are used to interpret the variety of findings within a unitary conceptual framework.

[1]  Lorraine G. Allan,et al.  Human contingency judgments: rule based or associative? , 1993, Psychological bulletin.

[2]  G. Logan Toward an instance theory of automatization. , 1988 .

[3]  Horst Müller,et al.  Komplexes Problemlösen: Reliabilität und Wissen , 1993 .

[4]  R. Nosofsky Choice, similarity, and the context theory of classification. , 1984 .

[5]  John R. Anderson,et al.  Causal inferences as perceptual judgments , 1995, Memory & cognition.

[6]  John R. Anderson,et al.  Rules of the Mind , 1993 .

[7]  Edgar Erdfelder,et al.  GPOWER: a priori, post-hoc, and compromise power analyses for MS-DOS [Computer Program] , 2004 .

[8]  D. Broadbent Levels, Hierarchies, and the Locus of Control* , 1977 .

[9]  Christian Lebiere,et al.  Implicit and explicit learning in ACT-R , 1998 .

[10]  John Sweller,et al.  Cognitive Load During Problem Solving: Effects on Learning , 1988, Cogn. Sci..

[11]  D. Broadbent,et al.  On the Relationship between Task Performance and Associated Verbalizable Knowledge , 1984 .

[12]  J. Gregory Trafton,et al.  Memory for Goals: An Architectural Perspective , 2020, Proceedings of the Twenty First Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society.

[13]  Bruce D. Burns,et al.  The Impact of Goal Specificity on Strategy Use and the Acquisition of Problem Structure , 1996, Cogn. Sci..

[14]  Douglas L. Medin,et al.  Context theory of classification learning. , 1978 .

[15]  Wolfgang Schoppek,et al.  Rules and Instances in the Control of a Static System-Modelling the Influence of Causal Interpretation , 1999 .

[16]  Joachim Funke,et al.  Finite-State Automata: Dynamic Task Environments in Problem-Solving Research , 1993 .

[17]  W. Ashby,et al.  Requisite Variety and Its Implications for the Control of Complex Systems , 1991 .

[18]  Z. Dienes,et al.  The role of specific instances in controlling a dynamic system , 1995 .

[19]  J. Funke Microworlds based on linear equation systems: A new approach to complex problem solving and experimental results , 1993 .

[20]  John R. Anderson The Architecture of Cognition , 1983 .

[21]  D. Broadbent,et al.  Interactive tasks and the implicit‐explicit distinction , 1988 .

[22]  D. Broadbent,et al.  Implicit and explicit knowledge in the control of complex systems , 1986 .

[23]  D. Plaut,et al.  Learning in Dynamic Decision Tasks: Computational Model and Empirical Evidence , 1997 .

[24]  C. Lebiere,et al.  The Atomic Components of Thought , 1998 .

[25]  John R. Anderson Cognitive psychology and its implications, 2nd ed. , 1985 .

[26]  L. Allan Human contingency judgments: rule based or associative? , 1993, Psychological bulletin.

[27]  Allen Newell,et al.  The psychology of human-computer interaction , 1983 .

[28]  Anas N. Al-Rabadi,et al.  A comparison of modified reconstructability analysis and Ashenhurst‐Curtis decomposition of Boolean functions , 2004 .

[29]  R. Nosofsky American Psychological Association, Inc. Choice, Similarity, and the Context Theory of Classification , 2022 .

[30]  Wiebke Putz-Osterloh Chapter 15 Strategies for Knowledge Acquisition and Transfer of Knowledge in Dynamic Tasks1 , 1993 .

[31]  Guy Karnas,et al.  Modes d'apprentissage sélectifs et non-sélectifs et connaissances acquises au contrôle d'un processus: évaluation d'un modèle simulé , 1989 .

[32]  R. Nosofsky,et al.  Rule-plus-exception model of classification learning. , 1994, Psychological review.