On the contribution of perceptual fluency and priming to recognition memory

Repetition priming has been shown to be independent of recognition memory. Thus, the severely amnesic patient E.P. has demonstrated intact stem completion priming and perceptual identification priming, despite at-chance performance on recognition memory tasks. It has also been shown that perceptual fluency can influence feelings of familiarity, in the sense that items perceived more quickly tend to be identified as familiar. If studied items are identified more fluently, due to perceptual priming, and fluency leads to familiarity, why do severely amnesic patients perform no better than chance on recognition memory tasks? One possibility is that severely amnesic patients do not exhibit normal fluency. Another possibility is that fluency is not a sufficiently strong cue for familiarity. In two experiments, 2 severely amnesic patients, 3 moderately amnesic patients, and 8 controls saw words slowly clearing from a mask. The participants identified each word as quickly as possible and then made a recognition (old/new) judgment. All the participants exhibited fluency, in that old responses were associated with shorter identification times than new responses were. In addition, for the severely amnesic patients, priming was intact, and recognition memory performance was at chance. We next calculated how much priming and fluency should elevate the probability of accurate recognition. The tendency to identify studied words rapidly (.6) and the tendency to label these rapidly identified words old (.6) would result in 36% of the studied words being labeled old. Other studied words were identified slowly (.4) but were still labeled old (.4), resulting in an additional 16% of studied words labeled old. Thus, the presence of fluency increases the probability of accurate recognition judgments to only 52% (chance = 50%). This finding explains why amnesic patients can exhibit both priming and fluency yet still perform at chance on recognition tests.

[1]  Gurindar S. Sohi,et al.  Memory systems , 1996, CSUR.

[2]  B. W. Whittlesea Illusions of familiarity. , 1993 .

[3]  Kevin J. Hawley,et al.  Contribution of perceptual fluency to recognition judgments. , 1991, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[4]  L. Squire,et al.  Intact Conceptual Priming in the Absence of Declarative Memory , 2004, Psychological science.

[5]  L R Squire,et al.  Intact perceptual memory in the absence of conscious memory. , 1997, Behavioral neuroscience.

[6]  S. Rajaram,et al.  Conceptual fluency selectively influences knowing. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[7]  J D Gabrieli,et al.  Double dissociation of memory capacities after bilateral occipital-lobe or medial temporal-lobe lesions. , 1995, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[8]  A. Ostergaard,et al.  Priming deficits in amnesia: Now you see them, now you don't , 1999, Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society.

[9]  Larry L. Jacoby,et al.  Illusions of immediate memory: evidence of an attributional basis for feelings of familiarity and perceptual quality , 1990 .

[10]  H. Kucera,et al.  Computational analysis of present-day American English , 1967 .

[11]  G. Mandler Recognizing: The judgment of previous occurrence. , 1980 .

[12]  N. Cohen From Conditioning to Conscious Recollection Memory Systems of the Brain. Oxford Psychology Series, Volume 35. , 2001 .

[13]  H. Eichenbaum,et al.  From Conditioning to Conscious Recollection , 2001 .

[14]  L. Squire Memory and the hippocampus: a synthesis from findings with rats, monkeys, and humans. , 1992, Psychological review.

[15]  J D Gabrieli,et al.  Dissociations between familiarity processes in explicit recognition and implicit perceptual memory. , 1997, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[16]  L. Jacoby,et al.  On the relationship between autobiographical memory and perceptual learning. , 1981, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[17]  R. Buckner,et al.  Dissociating Memory Retrieval Processes Using fMRI Evidence that Priming Does Not Support Recognition Memory , 2001, Neuron.

[18]  E. Tulving,et al.  Memory Systems 1994 , 1994 .

[19]  L. Squire,et al.  Recognition memory and familiarity judgments in severe amnesia: no evidence for a contribution of repetition priming. , 2000, Behavioral neuroscience.

[20]  E Tulving,et al.  Priming and human memory systems. , 1990, Science.

[21]  Debra A. Fleischman,et al.  Double Dissociation Between Memory Systems Underlying Explicit and Implicit Memory in the Human Brain , 1995 .

[22]  L. Squire Memory systems of the brain: A brief history and current perspective , 2004, Neurobiology of Learning and Memory.

[23]  Larry R Squire,et al.  Failure to acquire new semantic knowledge in patients with large medial temporal lobe lesions , 2005, Hippocampus.

[24]  D. Schacter,et al.  Priming and the Brain , 1998, Neuron.

[25]  Larry R Squire,et al.  Quantifying medial temporal lobe damage in memory‐impaired patients † , 2005, Hippocampus.

[26]  B. Knowlton,et al.  Learning and memory functions of the Basal Ganglia. , 2002, Annual review of neuroscience.

[27]  G D Logan,et al.  Fluency and response speed in recognition judgments , 1997, Memory & cognition.

[28]  L. Cermak,et al.  Perceptual fluency as a cue for recognition judgments in amnesia. , 1999, Neuropsychology.