A Program of Classical Conditioning Experiments Testing Variations in the Conditioned Stimulus and Context

Twenty-one experiments regarding the strength of attitudinal conditioning for various brands of cola were performed. The conditioned stimulus, brand familiarity (various unknown, moderately known, and well-known cola brands), and the embedding context In which conditioning trials occurred (other known or unknown brands) were manipulated. Effects are strongest for unknown and moderately known brands and for colas conditioned In a context of known versus unknown brands. Evidence is also provided showing that attitudes are conditioned only when subjects are aware of the contingency between conditioned and unconditioned stimuli. Copyright 1991 by the University of Chicago.

[1]  F. Baeyens,et al.  Contingency awareness in evaluative conditioning: A case for unaware affective-evaluative learning , 1990 .

[2]  A. Dickinson,et al.  Contingency awareness in evaluative conditioning: A comment on baeyens, eelen, and van den bergh , 1990 .

[3]  P. Reed,et al.  Context extinction following conditioning with delayed reward enhances subsequent instrumental responding. , 1990, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[4]  R. C. Honey,et al.  Contextual effects in conditioning, latent inhibition, and habituation: Associative and retrieval functions of contextual cues , 1989 .

[5]  Joan Meyers-Levy,et al.  Priming Effects on Product Judgments: A Hemispheric Interpretation , 1989 .

[6]  P. Herr Priming Price: Prior Knowledge and Context Effects , 1989 .

[7]  Anthony D. Cox,et al.  The Effects of Background Music in Advertising: A Reassessment , 1989 .

[8]  Chris Janiszewski,et al.  Assessing the Role of Contingency Awareness in Attitudinal Conditioning with Implications for Advertising Research , 1989 .

[9]  O. Van den Bergh,et al.  Acquired affective-evaluative value: conservative but not unchangeable. , 1989, Behaviour research and therapy.

[10]  K. Holyoak,et al.  A Theory of Conditioning: Inductive Learning within Rule-Based Default Hierarchies. , 1989 .

[11]  Frank Baeyens,et al.  Once in contact always in contact: Evaluative conditioning is resistant to extinction , 1988 .

[12]  R. Rescorla Pavlovian conditioning. It's not what you think it is. , 1988, The American psychologist.

[13]  Terence A. Shimp,et al.  Classical Conditioning of Consumer Attitudes: Four Experiments in an Advertising Context , 1987 .

[14]  Joel B. Cohen,et al.  Alternative Models of Categorization: Toward a Contingent Processing Framework , 1987 .

[15]  S. Beatty,et al.  Comment on Classically Conditioning Human Consumers , 1987 .

[16]  G. Gorn,et al.  Observations on Awareness and Conditioning , 1987 .

[17]  A. Schell Human autonomic and skeletal classical conditioning: The role of conscious cognitive factors. , 1987 .

[18]  M. Bouton,et al.  Analysis of the associative and occasion-setting properties of contexts participating in a Pavlovian discrimination. , 1986 .

[19]  R. Fazio,et al.  On the automatic activation of attitudes. , 1986, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[20]  N. Macklin Classical Conditioning Effects in Product/Character Pairings Presented to Children , 1986 .

[21]  Thomas J. Madden,et al.  A Closer Look at Classical Conditioning , 1985 .

[22]  Frances K. McSweeney,et al.  Classical Conditioning of Preferences for Stimuli , 1985 .

[23]  G. Albaum,et al.  A Meta-Analysis of Effect Sizes in Consumer Behavior Experiments , 1985 .

[24]  P. Balsam,et al.  Context and Learning , 1985 .

[25]  I. Martin,et al.  Conditioning, evaluations and cognitions: an axis of integration. , 1985, Behaviour research and therapy.

[26]  F. Mcsweeney,et al.  Recent Developments in Classical Conditioning , 1984 .

[27]  P. Holland Origins of Behavior in Pavlovian Conditioning , 1984 .

[28]  P. Herr,et al.  On the consequences of priming: Assimilation and contrast effects , 1983 .

[29]  M. Fredrikson,et al.  Pavlovian extinction, phobias, and the limits of the cognitive paradigm , 1983, The Pavlovian journal of biological science.

[30]  I. Martin,et al.  Part I. Cognitions, evaluations and conditioning: Rules of sequence and rules of consequence , 1983 .

[31]  Gregg B. Jackson,et al.  Meta-Analysis: Cumulating Research Findings Across Studies , 1982 .

[32]  R. Zajonc,et al.  Affective and Cognitive Factors in Preferences , 1982 .

[33]  E. Hirschman,et al.  The Experiential Aspects of Consumption: Consumer Fantasies, Feelings, and Fun , 1982 .

[34]  M. Dawson,et al.  Allocation of cognitive processing capacity during human autonomic classical conditioning. , 1982, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[35]  Gerald J. Gorn,et al.  The Effects of Music in Advertising on Choice Behavior: A Classical Conditioning Approach: , 1982 .

[36]  Joel B. Cohen The Role of Affect in Categorization: Toward a Reconsideration of the Concept of Attitude , 1982 .

[37]  M. Domjan The principles of learning and behavior , 1982 .

[38]  A G Barto,et al.  Toward a modern theory of adaptive networks: expectation and prediction. , 1981, Psychological review.

[39]  J. Cacioppo,et al.  Attitudes and Persuasion: Classic and Contemporary Approaches , 1981 .

[40]  R. Zajonc Feeling and thinking : Preferences need no inferences , 1980 .

[41]  J. Paul Peter,et al.  A Behavior Modification Perspective on Marketing , 1980 .

[42]  B. Schwartz Psychology of Learning and Behavior , 1978 .

[43]  W. Brewer There is no convincing evidence for operant or classical conditioning in adult humans. , 1974 .

[44]  A. Staats,et al.  Experimental demand characteristics and the classical conditioning of attitudes. , 1969 .

[45]  E. Hess Origins of Behavior , 1966 .

[46]  C. Staats,et al.  Attitudes established by classical conditioning. , 1958, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[47]  C. Staats,et al.  Meaning established by classical conditioning. , 1957, Journal of experimental psychology.