Probing the neural basis rational numbers: the role of inhibitory control and magnitude representations

Rational numbers, such as fractions, decimals and percentages, are a persistent challenge in the mathematics curriculum. An underappreciated source of rational number difficulties are whole number properties that apply to some, but not all, rational numbers. I contend that mastery of rational numbers involves refining and expanding whole number representations. Behavioral evidence for the role inhibitory control and magnitude-based processing of rational numbers support this hypothesis, although more attention is needed to task and stimuli selection, especially among fractions. In the brain, there is scant evidence on the role of inhibitory control in rational number processing, but surprisingly good correspondence, in the parietal lobe, between the handful of neuroimaging studies of rational numbers and the accumulated whole number literature. Decimals and discrete nonsymbolic representations are fruitful domains for probing the neural basis role of whole number interference in rational number processing.

[1]  S. Kripalani,et al.  Development and validation of the General Health Numeracy Test (GHNT). , 2013, Patient Education and Counseling.

[2]  David W. Braithwaite,et al.  Developmental changes in the whole number bias. , 2018, Developmental science.

[3]  Katherine Kipp,et al.  A Developmental Perspective on the Measurement of Cognitive Deficits in Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder , 2005, Biological Psychiatry.

[4]  Russell A. Poldrack,et al.  Large-scale automated synthesis of human functional neuroimaging data , 2011, Nature Methods.

[5]  Roberto A. Abreu-Mendoza,et al.  Children's discrete proportional reasoning is related to inhibitory control and enhanced by priming continuous representations. , 2020, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[6]  M. Barnes,et al.  A meta-analysis of mathematics and working memory: Moderating effects of working memory domain, type of mathematics skill, and sample characteristics , 2016 .

[7]  ROBERT S. MOYER,et al.  Time required for Judgements of Numerical Inequality , 1967, Nature.

[8]  Wim Fias,et al.  Common and distinct brain regions in both parietal and frontal cortex support symbolic and nonsymbolic number processing in humans: A functional neuroimaging meta-analysis , 2017, NeuroImage.

[9]  Chongde Lin,et al.  Flexible and unique representations of two-digit decimals. , 2014, Acta psychologica.

[10]  Sara Cordes,et al.  Attending to Relations: Proportional Reasoning in 3- to 6-Year-Old Children , 2017, Developmental psychology.

[11]  Andreas Obersteiner,et al.  Measuring fraction comparison strategies with eye-tracking , 2016 .

[12]  Yujing Ni,et al.  Teaching and Learning Fraction and Rational Numbers: The Origins and Implications of Whole Number Bias , 2005 .

[13]  L. Fuchs,et al.  Fraction Intervention for Students With Mathematics Difficulties: Lessons Learned From Five Randomized Controlled Trials , 2017, Journal of learning disabilities.

[14]  Lieven Verschaffel,et al.  Towards a mathematically more correct understanding of rational numbers: A longitudinal study with upper elementary school learners , 2018 .

[15]  Sashank Varma,et al.  Understanding decimal proportions: Discrete representations, parallel access, and privileged processing of zero , 2013, Cognitive Psychology.

[16]  A. Henik,et al.  Is three greater than five: The relation between physical and semantic size in comparison tasks , 1982, Memory & cognition.

[17]  Michael Schneider,et al.  Associations of non-symbolic and symbolic numerical magnitude processing with mathematical competence: a meta-analysis. , 2017, Developmental science.

[18]  Kerry Lee,et al.  Inhibition and Mathematical Performance: Poorly Correlated, Poorly Measured, or Poorly Matched? , 2018, Child Development Perspectives.

[19]  Lynn S. Fuchs,et al.  Does working memory moderate the effects of fraction intervention? An aptitude–treatment interaction. , 2014 .

[20]  Stephan Meier,et al.  Numerical ability predicts mortgage default , 2013, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[21]  K. Moeller,et al.  Processing symbolic and non-symbolic proportions: Domain-specific numerical and domain-general processes in intraparietal cortex , 2019, Brain Research.

[22]  S. Carey Précis of The Origin of Concepts , 2011, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[23]  Andreas Nieder,et al.  Tuning to non‐symbolic proportions in the human frontoparietal cortex , 2009, The European journal of neuroscience.

[24]  Susan C. Levine,et al.  The Development of Proportional Reasoning: Effect of Continuous Versus Discrete Quantities , 2007 .

[25]  M. J. Emerson,et al.  The Unity and Diversity of Executive Functions and Their Contributions to Complex “Frontal Lobe” Tasks: A Latent Variable Analysis , 2000, Cognitive Psychology.

[26]  A. Miyake,et al.  The relations among inhibition and interference control functions: a latent-variable analysis. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[27]  Hannah M. Nash,et al.  Are the literacy difficulties that characterize developmental dyslexia associated with a failure to integrate letters and speech sounds? , 2016, Developmental science.

[28]  Xinchen Fu,et al.  Inhibiting the Whole Number Bias in a Fraction Comparison Task: An Event-Related Potential Study , 2020, Psychology research and behavior management.

[29]  Daniel Ansari,et al.  Neural correlates of symbolic number processing in children and adults , 2005, Neuroreport.

[30]  Margarete Delazer,et al.  The processing and representation of fractions within the brain An fMRI investigation , 2009, NeuroImage.

[31]  M. Schocke,et al.  The processing and representation of fractions within the brain. An fMRI investigation. , 2009, NeuroImage.

[32]  Lynn S. Fuchs,et al.  General and math-specific predictors of sixth-graders’ knowledge of fractions ☆ , 2015 .

[33]  C. Eriksen,et al.  Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task , 1974 .

[34]  E. J. Carter,et al.  Functional Imaging of Numerical Processing in Adults and 4-y-Old Children , 2006, PLoS biology.

[35]  G. Scerif,et al.  Of Huge Mice and Tiny Elephants: Exploring the Relationship Between Inhibitory Processes and Preschool Math Skills , 2016, Front. Psychol..

[36]  Clarissa A. Thompson,et al.  An integrated theory of whole number and fractions development , 2011, Cognitive Psychology.

[37]  Dianne Davis,et al.  Association of Numeracy and Diabetes Control , 2008, Annals of Internal Medicine.

[38]  Ty W. Boyer,et al.  Development of proportional reasoning: where young children go wrong. , 2008, Developmental psychology.

[39]  Margot J. Taylor,et al.  Is 2+2=4? Meta-analyses of brain areas needed for numbers and calculations , 2011, NeuroImage.

[40]  Stella Vosniadou,et al.  The representation of fraction magnitudes and the whole number bias reconsidered. , 2015 .

[41]  P. H. Miller,et al.  A developmental perspective on executive function. , 2010, Child development.

[42]  K. K. Harnishfeger,et al.  Intending to forget: the development of cognitive inhibition in directed forgetting. , 1996, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[43]  Dallas Long,et al.  Students' Understanding of the Library of Congress Call Number System , 2013 .

[44]  Hong Li,et al.  An ERP study on the processing of common fractions , 2011, Experimental Brain Research.

[45]  Lieven Verschaffel,et al.  Naturally Biased? In Search for Reaction Time Evidence for a Natural Number Bias in Adults. , 2012 .

[46]  Martin H. Fischer,et al.  Extending the Mental Number Line A Review of Multi-Digit Number Processing , 2011 .

[47]  Martha W. Alibali,et al.  Knowledge Change as a Function of Mathematics Experience: All Contexts are Not Created Equal , 2005 .

[48]  B. Matthes,et al.  Collecting information on job tasks—an instrument to measure tasks required at the workplace in a multi-topic survey , 2014, Journal for Labour Market Research.

[49]  William W. Graves,et al.  Where is the semantic system? A critical review and meta-analysis of 120 functional neuroimaging studies. , 2009, Cerebral cortex.

[50]  Erno Lehtinen,et al.  Early Developmental Trajectories Toward Concepts of Rational Numbers , 2017 .

[51]  Lieven Verschaffel,et al.  Number sense in the transition from natural to rational numbers , 2017, The British journal of educational psychology.

[52]  Percival G. Matthews,et al.  Individual Differences in Nonsymbolic Ratio Processing Predict Symbolic Math Performance , 2016, Psychological science.

[53]  Luke F. Rinne,et al.  Pathways to fraction learning: Numerical abilities mediate the relation between early cognitive competencies and later fraction knowledge. , 2016, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[54]  S. Dehaene,et al.  A Magnitude Code Common to Numerosities and Number Symbols in Human Intraparietal Cortex , 2007, Neuron.

[55]  Charan Ranganath,et al.  Representational Similarity Analyses: A Practical Guide for Functional MRI Applications , 2018 .

[56]  K. Moeller,et al.  Magnitude processing of symbolic and non-symbolic proportions: an fMRI study , 2018, Behavioral and Brain Functions.

[57]  M. Handel What do people do at work? , 2016 .

[58]  P. Dartnell,et al.  Erratum to: The effect of inhibitory control on general mathematics achievement and fraction comparison in middle school children , 2015 .

[59]  Fruzsina Soltész,et al.  Event-related potentials dissociate facilitation and interference effects in the numerical Stroop paradigm , 2007, Neuropsychologia.

[60]  Keith J. Holyoak,et al.  Neural representations of magnitude for natural and rational numbers , 2016, NeuroImage.

[61]  Arthur J. Baroody,et al.  The Effects of Instruction on Children's Understanding of the "Equals" Sign , 1983, The Elementary School Journal.

[62]  M. Rosenberg-Lee Training Studies: An Experimental Design to Advance Educational Neuroscience. , 2018 .

[63]  Andreas Nieder,et al.  Notation-Independent Representation of Fractions in the Human Parietal Cortex , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[64]  Evelyn H. Kroesbergen,et al.  Working memory and mathematics in primary school children: A meta-analysis , 2013 .

[65]  Klaus Willmes,et al.  Decimal fraction representations are not distinct from natural number representations – evidence from a combined eye-tracking and computational modeling approach , 2014, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[66]  R. Siegler,et al.  Early Predictors of High School Mathematics Achievement , 2012, Psychological science.

[67]  V. Menon,et al.  Symbolic, numeric, and magnitude representations in the parietal cortex , 2009 .

[68]  Andreas Obersteiner,et al.  The natural number bias and magnitude representation in fraction comparison by expert mathematicians , 2013 .

[69]  Nikolaus Kriegeskorte,et al.  Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience Systems Neuroscience , 2022 .

[70]  Daniel Ansari,et al.  The role of the left intraparietal sulcus in the relationship between symbolic number processing and children's arithmetic competence , 2012, Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience.

[71]  Marie-Pascale Noël,et al.  Comparing the magnitude of two fractions with common components: which representations are used by 10- and 12-year-olds? , 2010, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[72]  Joseph Tzelgov,et al.  Decimals are not processed automatically, not even as being smaller than one. , 2014, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[73]  A. Diamond,et al.  An effect of inhibitory load in children while keeping working memory load constant , 2014, Front. Psychol..

[74]  Ty W. Boyer,et al.  Prompting children to reason proportionally: Processing discrete units as continuous amounts. , 2015, Developmental psychology.

[75]  B. Oken,et al.  EEG Frequency Changes Prior to Making Errors in an Easy Stroop Task , 2017, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[76]  C. Lonigan,et al.  Executive Function Among Preschool Children: Unitary Versus Distinct Abilities , 2014, Journal of psychopathology and behavioral assessment.

[77]  Marco Zorzi,et al.  The mental representation of numerical fractions: real or integer? , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[78]  O. Houdé,et al.  Inhibitory control and decimal number comparison in school-aged children , 2017, PloS one.

[79]  O. Houdé,et al.  Inhibition of the whole number bias in decimal number comparison: A developmental negative priming study. , 2019, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[80]  Daniel Ansari,et al.  Age-related Changes in the Activation of the Intraparietal Sulcus during Nonsymbolic Magnitude Processing: An Event-related Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Study , 2006, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.