Probing the neural basis rational numbers: the role of inhibitory control and magnitude representations
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] S. Kripalani,et al. Development and validation of the General Health Numeracy Test (GHNT). , 2013, Patient Education and Counseling.
[2] David W. Braithwaite,et al. Developmental changes in the whole number bias. , 2018, Developmental science.
[3] Katherine Kipp,et al. A Developmental Perspective on the Measurement of Cognitive Deficits in Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder , 2005, Biological Psychiatry.
[4] Russell A. Poldrack,et al. Large-scale automated synthesis of human functional neuroimaging data , 2011, Nature Methods.
[5] Roberto A. Abreu-Mendoza,et al. Children's discrete proportional reasoning is related to inhibitory control and enhanced by priming continuous representations. , 2020, Journal of experimental child psychology.
[6] M. Barnes,et al. A meta-analysis of mathematics and working memory: Moderating effects of working memory domain, type of mathematics skill, and sample characteristics , 2016 .
[7] ROBERT S. MOYER,et al. Time required for Judgements of Numerical Inequality , 1967, Nature.
[8] Wim Fias,et al. Common and distinct brain regions in both parietal and frontal cortex support symbolic and nonsymbolic number processing in humans: A functional neuroimaging meta-analysis , 2017, NeuroImage.
[9] Chongde Lin,et al. Flexible and unique representations of two-digit decimals. , 2014, Acta psychologica.
[10] Sara Cordes,et al. Attending to Relations: Proportional Reasoning in 3- to 6-Year-Old Children , 2017, Developmental psychology.
[11] Andreas Obersteiner,et al. Measuring fraction comparison strategies with eye-tracking , 2016 .
[12] Yujing Ni,et al. Teaching and Learning Fraction and Rational Numbers: The Origins and Implications of Whole Number Bias , 2005 .
[13] L. Fuchs,et al. Fraction Intervention for Students With Mathematics Difficulties: Lessons Learned From Five Randomized Controlled Trials , 2017, Journal of learning disabilities.
[14] Lieven Verschaffel,et al. Towards a mathematically more correct understanding of rational numbers: A longitudinal study with upper elementary school learners , 2018 .
[15] Sashank Varma,et al. Understanding decimal proportions: Discrete representations, parallel access, and privileged processing of zero , 2013, Cognitive Psychology.
[16] A. Henik,et al. Is three greater than five: The relation between physical and semantic size in comparison tasks , 1982, Memory & cognition.
[17] Michael Schneider,et al. Associations of non-symbolic and symbolic numerical magnitude processing with mathematical competence: a meta-analysis. , 2017, Developmental science.
[18] Kerry Lee,et al. Inhibition and Mathematical Performance: Poorly Correlated, Poorly Measured, or Poorly Matched? , 2018, Child Development Perspectives.
[19] Lynn S. Fuchs,et al. Does working memory moderate the effects of fraction intervention? An aptitude–treatment interaction. , 2014 .
[20] Stephan Meier,et al. Numerical ability predicts mortgage default , 2013, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[21] K. Moeller,et al. Processing symbolic and non-symbolic proportions: Domain-specific numerical and domain-general processes in intraparietal cortex , 2019, Brain Research.
[22] S. Carey. Précis of The Origin of Concepts , 2011, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[23] Andreas Nieder,et al. Tuning to non‐symbolic proportions in the human frontoparietal cortex , 2009, The European journal of neuroscience.
[24] Susan C. Levine,et al. The Development of Proportional Reasoning: Effect of Continuous Versus Discrete Quantities , 2007 .
[25] M. J. Emerson,et al. The Unity and Diversity of Executive Functions and Their Contributions to Complex “Frontal Lobe” Tasks: A Latent Variable Analysis , 2000, Cognitive Psychology.
[26] A. Miyake,et al. The relations among inhibition and interference control functions: a latent-variable analysis. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[27] Hannah M. Nash,et al. Are the literacy difficulties that characterize developmental dyslexia associated with a failure to integrate letters and speech sounds? , 2016, Developmental science.
[28] Xinchen Fu,et al. Inhibiting the Whole Number Bias in a Fraction Comparison Task: An Event-Related Potential Study , 2020, Psychology research and behavior management.
[29] Daniel Ansari,et al. Neural correlates of symbolic number processing in children and adults , 2005, Neuroreport.
[30] Margarete Delazer,et al. The processing and representation of fractions within the brain An fMRI investigation , 2009, NeuroImage.
[31] M. Schocke,et al. The processing and representation of fractions within the brain. An fMRI investigation. , 2009, NeuroImage.
[32] Lynn S. Fuchs,et al. General and math-specific predictors of sixth-graders’ knowledge of fractions ☆ , 2015 .
[33] C. Eriksen,et al. Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task , 1974 .
[34] E. J. Carter,et al. Functional Imaging of Numerical Processing in Adults and 4-y-Old Children , 2006, PLoS biology.
[35] G. Scerif,et al. Of Huge Mice and Tiny Elephants: Exploring the Relationship Between Inhibitory Processes and Preschool Math Skills , 2016, Front. Psychol..
[36] Clarissa A. Thompson,et al. An integrated theory of whole number and fractions development , 2011, Cognitive Psychology.
[37] Dianne Davis,et al. Association of Numeracy and Diabetes Control , 2008, Annals of Internal Medicine.
[38] Ty W. Boyer,et al. Development of proportional reasoning: where young children go wrong. , 2008, Developmental psychology.
[39] Margot J. Taylor,et al. Is 2+2=4? Meta-analyses of brain areas needed for numbers and calculations , 2011, NeuroImage.
[40] Stella Vosniadou,et al. The representation of fraction magnitudes and the whole number bias reconsidered. , 2015 .
[41] P. H. Miller,et al. A developmental perspective on executive function. , 2010, Child development.
[42] K. K. Harnishfeger,et al. Intending to forget: the development of cognitive inhibition in directed forgetting. , 1996, Journal of experimental child psychology.
[43] Dallas Long,et al. Students' Understanding of the Library of Congress Call Number System , 2013 .
[44] Hong Li,et al. An ERP study on the processing of common fractions , 2011, Experimental Brain Research.
[45] Lieven Verschaffel,et al. Naturally Biased? In Search for Reaction Time Evidence for a Natural Number Bias in Adults. , 2012 .
[46] Martin H. Fischer,et al. Extending the Mental Number Line A Review of Multi-Digit Number Processing , 2011 .
[47] Martha W. Alibali,et al. Knowledge Change as a Function of Mathematics Experience: All Contexts are Not Created Equal , 2005 .
[48] B. Matthes,et al. Collecting information on job tasks—an instrument to measure tasks required at the workplace in a multi-topic survey , 2014, Journal for Labour Market Research.
[49] William W. Graves,et al. Where is the semantic system? A critical review and meta-analysis of 120 functional neuroimaging studies. , 2009, Cerebral cortex.
[50] Erno Lehtinen,et al. Early Developmental Trajectories Toward Concepts of Rational Numbers , 2017 .
[51] Lieven Verschaffel,et al. Number sense in the transition from natural to rational numbers , 2017, The British journal of educational psychology.
[52] Percival G. Matthews,et al. Individual Differences in Nonsymbolic Ratio Processing Predict Symbolic Math Performance , 2016, Psychological science.
[53] Luke F. Rinne,et al. Pathways to fraction learning: Numerical abilities mediate the relation between early cognitive competencies and later fraction knowledge. , 2016, Journal of experimental child psychology.
[54] S. Dehaene,et al. A Magnitude Code Common to Numerosities and Number Symbols in Human Intraparietal Cortex , 2007, Neuron.
[55] Charan Ranganath,et al. Representational Similarity Analyses: A Practical Guide for Functional MRI Applications , 2018 .
[56] K. Moeller,et al. Magnitude processing of symbolic and non-symbolic proportions: an fMRI study , 2018, Behavioral and Brain Functions.
[57] M. Handel. What do people do at work? , 2016 .
[58] P. Dartnell,et al. Erratum to: The effect of inhibitory control on general mathematics achievement and fraction comparison in middle school children , 2015 .
[59] Fruzsina Soltész,et al. Event-related potentials dissociate facilitation and interference effects in the numerical Stroop paradigm , 2007, Neuropsychologia.
[60] Keith J. Holyoak,et al. Neural representations of magnitude for natural and rational numbers , 2016, NeuroImage.
[61] Arthur J. Baroody,et al. The Effects of Instruction on Children's Understanding of the "Equals" Sign , 1983, The Elementary School Journal.
[62] M. Rosenberg-Lee. Training Studies: An Experimental Design to Advance Educational Neuroscience. , 2018 .
[63] Andreas Nieder,et al. Notation-Independent Representation of Fractions in the Human Parietal Cortex , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[64] Evelyn H. Kroesbergen,et al. Working memory and mathematics in primary school children: A meta-analysis , 2013 .
[65] Klaus Willmes,et al. Decimal fraction representations are not distinct from natural number representations – evidence from a combined eye-tracking and computational modeling approach , 2014, Front. Hum. Neurosci..
[66] R. Siegler,et al. Early Predictors of High School Mathematics Achievement , 2012, Psychological science.
[67] V. Menon,et al. Symbolic, numeric, and magnitude representations in the parietal cortex , 2009 .
[68] Andreas Obersteiner,et al. The natural number bias and magnitude representation in fraction comparison by expert mathematicians , 2013 .
[69] Nikolaus Kriegeskorte,et al. Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience Systems Neuroscience , 2022 .
[70] Daniel Ansari,et al. The role of the left intraparietal sulcus in the relationship between symbolic number processing and children's arithmetic competence , 2012, Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience.
[71] Marie-Pascale Noël,et al. Comparing the magnitude of two fractions with common components: which representations are used by 10- and 12-year-olds? , 2010, Journal of experimental child psychology.
[72] Joseph Tzelgov,et al. Decimals are not processed automatically, not even as being smaller than one. , 2014, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[73] A. Diamond,et al. An effect of inhibitory load in children while keeping working memory load constant , 2014, Front. Psychol..
[74] Ty W. Boyer,et al. Prompting children to reason proportionally: Processing discrete units as continuous amounts. , 2015, Developmental psychology.
[75] B. Oken,et al. EEG Frequency Changes Prior to Making Errors in an Easy Stroop Task , 2017, Front. Hum. Neurosci..
[76] C. Lonigan,et al. Executive Function Among Preschool Children: Unitary Versus Distinct Abilities , 2014, Journal of psychopathology and behavioral assessment.
[77] Marco Zorzi,et al. The mental representation of numerical fractions: real or integer? , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[78] O. Houdé,et al. Inhibitory control and decimal number comparison in school-aged children , 2017, PloS one.
[79] O. Houdé,et al. Inhibition of the whole number bias in decimal number comparison: A developmental negative priming study. , 2019, Journal of experimental child psychology.
[80] Daniel Ansari,et al. Age-related Changes in the Activation of the Intraparietal Sulcus during Nonsymbolic Magnitude Processing: An Event-related Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Study , 2006, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.