Debunking vaccination myths: strong risk negations can increase perceived vaccination risks.

OBJECTIVE Information about risks is often contradictory, especially in the health domain. A vast amount of bizarre information on vaccine-adverse events (VAE) can be found on the Internet; most are posted by antivaccination activists. Several actors in the health sector struggle against these statements by negating claimed risks with scientific explanations. The goal of the present work is to find optimal ways of negating risk to decrease risk perceptions. METHODS In two online experiments, we varied the extremity of risk negations and their source. Perception of the probability of VAE, their expected severity (both variables serve as indicators of perceived risk), and vaccination intentions. RESULTS Paradoxically, messages strongly indicating that there is "no risk" led to a higher perceived vaccination risk than weak negations. This finding extends previous work on the negativity bias, which has shown that information stating the presence of risk decreases risk perceptions, while information negating the existence of risk increases such perceptions. Several moderators were also tested; however, the effect occurred independently of the number of negations, recipient involvement, and attitude. Solely the credibility of the information source interacted with the extremity of risk negation: For credible sources (governmental institutions), strong and weak risk negations lead to similar perceived risk, while for less credible sources (pharmaceutical industries) weak negations lead to less perceived risk than strong negations. CONCLUSIONS Optimal risk negation may profit from moderate rather than extreme formulations as a source's trustworthiness can vary.

[1]  J. Zaichkowsky Conceptualizing Involvement , 1986 .

[2]  I. Ajzen The theory of planned behavior , 1991 .

[3]  Neil D. Weinstein,et al.  Testing four competing theories of health-protective behavior. , 1993 .

[4]  Paul Slovic,et al.  Perceived risk, trust, and democracy , 1993 .

[5]  N. Weinstein Testing four competing theories of health-protective behavior. , 1993, Health psychology : official journal of the Division of Health Psychology, American Psychological Association.

[6]  J. Sinacore Multiple regression: Testing and interpreting interactions , 1993 .

[7]  D G MacGregor,et al.  Perception of Risks from Electromagnetic Fields: A Psychometric Evaluation of a Risk-Communication Approach , 1994, Risk analysis : an official publication of the Society for Risk Analysis.

[8]  Helmut Jungermann,et al.  Credibility, Information Preferences, and Information Interests , 1995 .

[9]  E. Weber,et al.  Perceived risk attitudes: relating risk perception to risky choice , 1997 .

[10]  V T Covello,et al.  The Determinants of Trust and Credibility in Environmental Risk Communication: An Empirical Study , 1997, Risk analysis : an official publication of the Society for Risk Analysis.

[11]  A. Meta-analysis,et al.  The Influence of Framing on Risky Decisions , 1998 .

[12]  Mark P. Zanna,et al.  On the Primacy of Affect in the Determination of Attitudes and Behavior: The Moderating Role of Affective-Cognitive Ambivalence , 1998 .

[13]  A. Wakefield,et al.  Ileal-lymphoid-nodular hyperplasia, non-specific colitis, and pervasive developmental disorder in children. , 1998, Lancet.

[14]  Brad J. Bushman,et al.  Effects of Warning and Information Labels on Consumption of Full-Fat, Reduced-Fat, and No-Fat Products , 1998 .

[15]  A. Kühberger,et al.  The Influence of Framing on Risky Decisions: A Meta-analysis. , 1998, Organizational behavior and human decision processes.

[16]  F. Gibbons,et al.  The effect of risk communication on risk perceptions: the significance of individual differences. , 1999, Journal of the National Cancer Institute. Monographs.

[17]  N D Weinstein,et al.  Perceived probability, perceived severity, and health-protective behavior. , 2000, Health psychology : official journal of the Division of Health Psychology, American Psychological Association.

[18]  J. Hammitt,et al.  Perceived Risks of Conventional and Organic Produce: Pesticides, Pathogens, and Natural Toxins , 2001, Risk analysis : an official publication of the Society for Risk Analysis.

[19]  E. Ernst Rise in popularity of complementary and alternative medicine: reasons and consequences for vaccination. , 2001, Vaccine.

[20]  Edward B. Royzman,et al.  Negativity Bias, Negativity Dominance, and Contagion , 2001 .

[21]  Christopher K. Hsee,et al.  Risk as Feelings , 2001, Psychological bulletin.

[22]  M. Siegrist,et al.  Better Negative than Positive? Evidence of a Bias for Negative Information about Possible Health Dangers , 2001, Risk analysis : an official publication of the Society for Risk Analysis.

[23]  R. Petty,et al.  What doesn't kill me makes me stronger: the effects of resisting persuasion on attitude certainty. , 2002, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[24]  Sabine Pahl,et al.  Trust in Risky Messages: The Role of Prior Attitudes , 2003, Risk analysis : an official publication of the Society for Risk Analysis.

[25]  R. Petty,et al.  The Influence of Spokesperson Trustworthiness on Message Elaboration, Attitude Strength, and Advertising Effectiveness , 2003 .

[26]  M. Ness,et al.  Public worry about specific food safety issues , 2004 .

[27]  Ortwin Renn,et al.  Perception of risks. , 2004, Toxicology letters.

[28]  L. Moulton,et al.  Factors associated with refusal of childhood vaccines among parents of school-aged children: a case-control study. , 2005, Archives of pediatrics & adolescent medicine.

[29]  G. Chapman,et al.  Moderators of the intention–behavior relationship in influenza vaccinations: Intention stability and unforeseen barriers , 2005 .

[30]  Bertram Gawronski,et al.  At the boundaries of automaticity: negation as reflective operation. , 2006, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[31]  JULIE POVALL,et al.  Risk Appraisal , 2006 .

[32]  N. Weinstein,et al.  Risk perceptions: assessment and relationship to influenza vaccination. , 2007, Health psychology : official journal of the Division of Health Psychology, American Psychological Association.

[33]  M. A. Gerend,et al.  Using message framing to promote acceptance of the human papillomavirus vaccine. , 2007, Health psychology : official journal of the Division of Health Psychology, American Psychological Association.

[34]  Noel T Brewer,et al.  Meta-analysis of the relationship between risk perception and health behavior: the example of vaccination. , 2007, Health psychology : official journal of the Division of Health Psychology, American Psychological Association.

[35]  D. Glik Risk communication for public health emergencies. , 2007, Annual review of public health.

[36]  Bertram Gawronski,et al.  When “Just Say No” is not enough: Affirmation versus negation training and the reduction of automatic stereotype activation☆ , 2008 .

[37]  Stephen J. Thoma,et al.  The Challenge of Measuring Epistemic Beliefs: An Analysis of Three Self-Report Instruments , 2008 .

[38]  R. Petty,et al.  Persuasion: From Single to Multiple to Metacognitive Processes , 2008, Perspectives on psychological science : a journal of the Association for Psychological Science.

[39]  M. Gnädinger,et al.  Attitudes towards vaccination: users of complementary and alternative medicine versus non-users. , 2009, Swiss medical weekly.

[40]  Erika A. Waters,et al.  Explanations for side effect aversion in preventive medical treatment decisions. , 2009, Health psychology : official journal of the Division of Health Psychology, American Psychological Association.

[41]  Baruch Fischhoff 8.8 Risk perception and communication , 2009 .

[42]  S. Nestler Belief Perseverance: The Role of Accessible Content and Accessibility Experiences , 2010 .

[43]  G. Godin,et al.  Determinants of Influenza Vaccination among Healthcare Workers , 2010, Infection Control & Hospital Epidemiology.

[44]  C. Betsch,et al.  Der Einfluss von Furchtappellen im Kontext impfkritischer Internetseiten , 2010 .

[45]  A. Kata A postmodern Pandora's box: anti-vaccination misinformation on the Internet. , 2010, Vaccine.

[46]  Uma R. Karmarkar,et al.  Believe Me, I Have No Idea What I’m Talking About: The Effects of Source Certainty on Consumer Involvement and Persuasion , 2010 .

[47]  T. Lancet,et al.  Retraction—Ileal-lymphoid-nodular hyperplasia, non-specific colitis, and pervasive developmental disorder in children , 2010, The Lancet.

[48]  Frank Renkewitz,et al.  The Influence of Vaccine-critical Websites on Perceiving Vaccination Risks , 2010, Journal of health psychology.

[49]  C. Betsch,et al.  Der Einfluss von Furchtappellen im Kontext impfkritischer Internetseiten , 2010, Kinder- und Jugendmedizin.

[50]  Ch Hsu,et al.  Risk communication and college students: the 2009 H1N1 pandemic influenza. , 2011, Preventive medicine.

[51]  A. Gilsdorf,et al.  Update on the ongoing outbreak of haemolytic uraemic syndrome due to Shiga toxin-producing Escherichia coli (STEC) serotype O104, Germany, May 2011. , 2011, Euro surveillance : bulletin Europeen sur les maladies transmissibles = European communicable disease bulletin.

[52]  G Krause,et al.  Large and ongoing outbreak of haemolytic uraemic syndrome, Germany, May 2011. , 2011, Euro surveillance : bulletin Europeen sur les maladies transmissibles = European communicable disease bulletin.

[53]  Frank Renkewitz,et al.  The Influence of Narrative v. Statistical Information on Perceiving Vaccination Risks , 2011, Medical decision making : an international journal of the Society for Medical Decision Making.

[54]  L. Mckenna,et al.  The use of complementary and alternative medicine by pregnant women: a literature review. , 2011, Midwifery.

[55]  M. Siegrist,et al.  Measuring people's knowledge about vaccination: developing a one-dimensional scale. , 2012, Vaccine.