Explanation of Fitts’ law in Reaching Movement based on Human Arm Dynamics

Why does Fitts’ law fit various human behavioural data well even though it is not a model based on human physical dynamics? To clarify this, we derived the relationships among the factors applied in Fitts’ law—movement duration and spatial endpoint error—based on a multi-joint forward- and inverse-dynamics models in the presence of signal-dependent noise. As a result, the relationship between them was modelled as an inverse proportion. To validate whether the endpoint error calculated by the model can represent the endpoint error of actual movements, we conducted a behavioural experiment in which centre-out reaching movements were performed under temporal constraints in four directions using the shoulder and elbow joints. The result showed that the distributions of model endpoint error closely expressed the observed endpoint error distributions. Furthermore, the model was found to be nearly consistent with Fitts’ law. Further analysis revealed that the coefficients of Fitts’ law could be expressed by arm dynamics and signal-dependent noise parameters. Consequently, our answer to the question above is: Fitts’ law for reaching movements can be expressed based on human arm dynamics; thus, Fitts’ law closely fits human’s behavioural data under various conditions.

[1]  Robert Sessions Woodworth,et al.  THE ACCURACY OF VOLUNTARY MOVEMENT , 1899 .

[2]  P. Fitts The information capacity of the human motor system in controlling the amplitude of movement. , 1954, Journal of experimental psychology.

[3]  P. Fitts,et al.  INFORMATION CAPACITY OF DISCRETE MOTOR RESPONSES. , 1964, Journal of experimental psychology.

[4]  A. H. Norris,et al.  Speed and accuracy of movement and their changes with age. , 1969, Acta psychologica.

[5]  D. Chaffin,et al.  An investigation of fitts' law using a wide range of movement amplitudes. , 1976, Journal of motor behavior.

[6]  Timothy D. Lee,et al.  Motor Control and Learning: A Behavioral Emphasis , 1982 .

[7]  E. R. Crossman,et al.  Feedback Control of Hand-Movement and Fitts' Law , 1983, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[8]  B. Kelso The effects of extended practice on aiming movements in terms of Fitts' Law , 1984 .

[9]  R J Jagacinski,et al.  Fitts' Law in two dimensions with hand and head movements. , 1983, Journal of motor behavior.

[10]  C. Atkeson,et al.  Kinematic features of unrestrained vertical arm movements , 1985, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[11]  R A Abrams,et al.  Optimality in human motor performance: ideal control of rapid aimed movements. , 1988, Psychological review.

[12]  宇野 洋二,et al.  Formation and control of optimal trajectory in human multijoint arm movement : minimum torque-change model , 1988 .

[13]  I. Scott MacKenzie,et al.  Extending Fitts' law to two-dimensional tasks , 1992, CHI.

[14]  N L Goggin,et al.  Age-related differences in the control of spatial aiming movements. , 1992, Research quarterly for exercise and sport.

[15]  Gerard P. van Galen,et al.  Fitts' law as the outcome of a dynamic noise filtering model of motor control , 1995 .

[16]  A. D. Fisk,et al.  Age-related differences in movement control: adjusting submovement structure to optimize performance. , 1997, The journals of gerontology. Series B, Psychological sciences and social sciences.

[17]  R Plamondon,et al.  Speed/accuracy trade-offs in target-directed movements , 1997, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[18]  Daniel M. Wolpert,et al.  Making smooth moves , 2022 .

[19]  H. Gomi,et al.  Task-Dependent Viscoelasticity of Human Multijoint Arm and Its Spatial Characteristics for Interaction with Environments , 1998, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[20]  Atsuo Murata,et al.  Extending Effective Target Width in Fitts' Law to a Two-Dimensional Pointing Task , 1999, Int. J. Hum. Comput. Interact..

[21]  Y Uno,et al.  Quantitative examinations of internal representations for arm trajectory planning: minimum commanded torque change model. , 1999, Journal of neurophysiology.

[22]  Denis Mottet,et al.  The dynamics of goal-directed rhythmical aiming , 1999, Biological Cybernetics.

[23]  M. Kawato,et al.  Formation and control of optimal trajectory in human multijoint arm movement , 1989, Biological Cybernetics.

[24]  M. Kawato,et al.  Optimal impedance control for task achievement in the presence of signal-dependent noise. , 2004, Journal of neurophysiology.

[25]  R. J. van Beers,et al.  The role of execution noise in movement variability. , 2004, Journal of neurophysiology.

[26]  Yves Guiard,et al.  Fitts' law in two-dimensional task space , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[27]  Mitsuo Kawato,et al.  A via-point time optimization algorithm for complex sequential trajectory formation , 2004, Neural Networks.

[28]  Yasuharu Koike,et al.  Estimation of dynamic joint torques and trajectory formation from surface electromyography signals using a neural network model , 1995, Biological Cybernetics.

[29]  Ning Qian,et al.  An optimization principle for determining movement duration. , 2006, Journal of neurophysiology.

[30]  Errol R Hoffmann,et al.  Movement times of different arm components , 2010, Ergonomics.

[31]  M. L. Latash,et al.  Fitts’ Law in early postural adjustments , 2013, Neuroscience.