The Suppression of Irrelevant Semantic Representations in Parkinson’s Disease

The impairment of lexical-semantic inhibition mechanisms in Parkinson’s disease (PD) remains a source of contention. In order to observe whether people with PD are able to suppress irrelevant semantic information during picture naming, the present study employed an object-based negative priming paradigm with 16 participants with PD and 13 healthy controls. The task required participants to name a red target image while ignoring a superimposed, green distractor image. The semantic relationship between the distractor image and the target image of the subsequent trial was manipulated, such that the distractor image was identical, semantically related, or semantically unrelated to said target image. The PD group and the control group were slower in naming a target image that had previously served as a distractor image, relative to naming a target image that was unrelated to the previous distractor image. Thus, a negative priming effect was present in both groups. Furthermore, no significant difference in the magnitude of this effect was observed between the control and PD groups. When considered in the context of existing literature surrounding negative priming in PD, these results suggest that inhibition is subserved by multiple, domain-specific mechanisms and that the inhibitory processing of visual-semantic stimuli is intact in PD.

[1]  J. Kulisevsky,et al.  Parkinson's Disease-Cognitive Rating Scale , 2020 .

[2]  B. Kopp,et al.  Neural mechanisms underlying cognitive inflexibility in Parkinson's disease , 2016, Neuropsychologia.

[3]  Randi C. Martin,et al.  Selective inhibition and naming performance in semantic blocking, picture-word interference, and color-word Stroop tasks. , 2015, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[4]  E. Fox,et al.  The negative priming paradigm: An update and implications for selective attention , 2015, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[5]  V. Trevisani,et al.  Effects of external cues on gait parameters of Parkinson's disease patients: A systematic review , 2014, Clinical Neurology and Neurosurgery.

[6]  Rutvik H. Desai,et al.  Parkinson’s disease disrupts both automatic and controlled processing of action verbs , 2013, Brain and Language.

[7]  M. Jenkins,et al.  Cueing and gait improvement among people with Parkinson's disease: a meta-analysis. , 2013, Archives of physical medicine and rehabilitation.

[8]  Fernando Cuetos,et al.  Verbal fluency in Parkinson’s disease patients on/off dopamine medication , 2012, Neuropsychologia.

[9]  J. Michael Herrmann,et al.  Identity Negative Priming: A Phenomenon of Perception, Recognition or Selection? , 2012, PloS one.

[10]  P. Silburn,et al.  Assessment methods and factors associated with depression in Parkinson's disease , 2011, Journal of the Neurological Sciences.

[11]  B. Murdoch,et al.  The Influence of Dopamine on Automatic and Controlled Semantic Activation in Parkinson's Disease , 2011, Parkinson's disease.

[12]  L. Clare,et al.  Executive functions in Parkinson's disease: Systematic review and meta‐analysis , 2011, Movement disorders : official journal of the Movement Disorder Society.

[13]  M. Weiner,et al.  Normative data for the Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA) in a population-based sample , 2011, Neurology.

[14]  Marjan Jahanshahi,et al.  Deficits in inhibitory control and conflict resolution on cognitive and motor tasks in Parkinson’s disease , 2011, Experimental Brain Research.

[15]  C. Clarke,et al.  Systematic review of levodopa dose equivalency reporting in Parkinson's disease , 2010, Movement disorders : official journal of the Movement Disorder Society.

[16]  B. Murdoch,et al.  Decreased semantic competitive inhibition in Parkinson's disease: Evidence from an investigation of word search performance , 2010, International journal of speech-language pathology.

[17]  Manuel Menéndez,et al.  Action naming is impaired in Parkinson disease patients , 2009, Neuropsychologia.

[18]  B. Murdoch,et al.  Semantic activation in Parkinson's disease patients on and off levodopa , 2009, Cortex.

[19]  Jaime Kulisevsky,et al.  Cognitive impairment in Parkinson's disease: Tools for diagnosis and assessment , 2009, Movement disorders : official journal of the Movement Disorder Society.

[20]  M. Jeannerod,et al.  Word processing in Parkinson's disease is impaired for action verbs but not for concrete nouns , 2008, Neuropsychologia.

[21]  D. Beversdorf,et al.  Dopaminergic Modulation of Semantic Priming in Parkinson Disease , 2008, Cognitive and behavioral neurology : official journal of the Society for Behavioral and Cognitive Neurology.

[22]  Jaime Kulisevsky,et al.  Parkinson's disease‐cognitive rating scale: A new cognitive scale specific for Parkinson's disease , 2008, Movement disorders : official journal of the Movement Disorder Society.

[23]  S. Cappa,et al.  Action and object naming in Parkinson's disease without dementia , 2007, European journal of neurology.

[24]  Joanna E. Castner,et al.  Semantic and affective priming as a function of stimulation of the subthalamic nucleus in Parkinson's disease. , 2007, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[25]  P. Silburn,et al.  Validity of Hamilton depression inventory in Parkinson's disease , 2007, Movement disorders : official journal of the Movement Disorder Society.

[26]  H. Gibbons,et al.  Unimpaired negative but enhanced positive priming in Parkinson's disease: Evidence from an identity and a location priming task , 2006, Neuropsychologia.

[27]  P. Praamstra,et al.  Time-course of masked response priming and inhibition in Parkinson's disease , 2006, Neuropsychologia.

[28]  R. Cools Dopaminergic modulation of cognitive function-implications for l-DOPA treatment in Parkinson's disease , 2006, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[29]  Katie L. McMahon,et al.  Classic identity negative priming involves accessing semantic representations in the left anterior temporal cortex , 2006, NeuroImage.

[30]  Kenneth M Heilman,et al.  Visual selective attention in Parkinson's disease: dissociation of exogenous and endogenous inhibition. , 2006, Neuropsychology.

[31]  David A Copland,et al.  The influence of dopamine on semantic activation in Parkinson's disease: evidence from a multipriming task. , 2006, Neuropsychology.

[32]  David L. Strayer,et al.  Lack of Impairment in Patients with Parkinson's Disease on an Object-Based Negative Priming Task , 2006, Perceptual and motor skills.

[33]  H. Bokura,et al.  Event-related potentials for response inhibition in Parkinson's disease , 2005, Neuropsychologia.

[34]  A. Hayes,et al.  Lack of inhibition in Parkinson's disease: evidence from a lexical decision task , 2005, Neuropsychologia.

[35]  Mathias Pessiglione,et al.  An Effect of Dopamine Depletion on Decision-making: The Temporal Coupling of Deliberation and Execution , 2005, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[36]  G. Kwakkel,et al.  Effects of external rhythmical cueing on gait in patients with Parkinson's disease: a systematic review , 2005, Clinical rehabilitation.

[37]  B. Murdoch,et al.  Summation of semantic priming and complex sentence comprehension in Parkinson's disease. , 2005, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[38]  Antje S Meyer,et al.  Processing of extrafoveal objects during multiple-object naming. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[39]  Angela R Laird,et al.  Meta‐analyses of object naming: Effect of baseline , 2005, Human brain mapping.

[40]  J. Cummings,et al.  The Montreal Cognitive Assessment, MoCA: A Brief Screening Tool For Mild Cognitive Impairment , 2005, Journal of the American Geriatrics Society.

[41]  C. Davis N-Watch: A program for deriving neighborhood size and other psycholinguistic statistics , 2005, Behavior research methods.

[42]  Daniel Herron,et al.  A new on-line resource for psycholinguistic studies. , 2004, Journal of memory and language.

[43]  J. Henry,et al.  Verbal fluency deficits in Parkinson's disease: A meta-analysis , 2004, Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society.

[44]  M. Rieger,et al.  Inhibition of ongoing responses in patients with Parkinson’s disease , 2004, Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry.

[45]  M. Petrides,et al.  Neural Bases of Set-Shifting Deficits in Parkinson's Disease , 2004, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[46]  D. Copland The basal ganglia and semantic engagement: Potential insights from semantic priming in individuals with subcortical vascular lesions, Parkinson's disease, and cortical lesions , 2003, Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society.

[47]  Jennifer D. Davis,et al.  Semantic and Cross-Case Identity Priming in Patients With Parkinson's Disease , 2003, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[48]  D. Bub,et al.  Task switching deficits associated with Parkinson’s disease reflect depleted attentional resources , 2002, Neuropsychologia.

[49]  David L. Strayer,et al.  Negative priming in patients with Parkinson's disease: evidence for a role of the striatum in inhibitory attentional processes. , 2002, Neuropsychology.

[50]  T. Robbins,et al.  Enhanced or impaired cognitive function in Parkinson's disease as a function of dopaminergic medication and task demands. , 2001, Cerebral cortex.

[51]  E. Siemers,et al.  Influence of Competing Distractors on Response Selection in Huntington's Disease and Parkinson's Disease , 2001, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[52]  B. Murdoch,et al.  Semantic Priming in Parkinsons Disease: Evidence for Delayed Spreading Activation , 2001, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[53]  G. Humphreys,et al.  Hierarchies, similarity, and interactivity in object recognition: “Category-specific” neuropsychological deficits , 2001, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[54]  Markus F Damian,et al.  Semantic negative priming in picture categorization and naming , 2000, Cognition.

[55]  M. J. Emerson,et al.  The Unity and Diversity of Executive Functions and Their Contributions to Complex “Frontal Lobe” Tasks: A Latent Variable Analysis , 2000, Cognitive Psychology.

[56]  Jonathan Grainger,et al.  Unconscious semantic priming from pictures , 1999, Cognition.

[57]  A. Tröster,et al.  Lexical, semantic, and action verbal fluency in Parkinson's disease with and without dementia. , 1999, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[58]  Jeffrey S. Bowers,et al.  Grossberg and colleagues solved the hyperonym problem over a decade ago , 1999, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[59]  David P. Salmon,et al.  Cortical and subcortical influences on clustering and switching in the performance of verbal fluency tasks , 1998, Neuropsychologia.

[60]  A E Seiffert,et al.  Selective attention: a reevaluation of the implications of negative priming. , 1998, Psychological review.

[61]  M. Hoehn,et al.  Parkinsonism , 1998, Neurology.

[62]  J. Gorell,et al.  Impaired set-shifting in Parkinson's disease: new evidence from a lexical decision task. , 1996, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[63]  M. Spitzer,et al.  Dopaminergic modulation of semantic network activation , 1996, Neuropsychologia.

[64]  J. Gurd,et al.  Competitive Inhibition Models of Lexical–Semantic Processing: Experimental Evidence , 1996, Brain and Language.

[65]  Willem J. M. Levelt,et al.  A theory of lexical access in speech production , 1999, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[66]  Max Coltheart,et al.  Psycholinguistic assessments of language processing in aphasia (PALPA) , 1996 .

[67]  E. Fox Negative priming from ignored distractors in visual selection: A review , 1995, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[68]  Avishai Henik,et al.  Disinhibition of Automatic Word Reading in Parkinson's Disease , 1993, Cortex.

[69]  M. Grossman,et al.  Verbal fluency deficits in Parkinson's disease. , 1993 .

[70]  Lesley Milroy,et al.  Linguistics and Aphasia: Psycholinguistic and Pragmatic Aspects of Intervention , 1993 .

[71]  A. Roelofs,et al.  A spreading-activation theory of lemma retrieval in speaking , 1992, Cognition.

[72]  C. Marsden,et al.  Internal versus external cues and the control of attention in Parkinson's disease. , 1988, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[73]  J. Yesavage,et al.  Geriatric Depression Scale (GDS): Recent evidence and development of a shorter version. , 1986 .

[74]  S. Tipper The Negative Priming Effect: Inhibitory Priming by Ignored Objects , 1985, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[75]  Allan Collins,et al.  A spreading-activation theory of semantic processing , 1975 .

[76]  M. Annett A classification of hand preference by association analysis. , 1970, British journal of psychology.

[77]  M. Hoehn,et al.  Parkinsonism , 1967, Neurology.

[78]  Steven P. Tipper,et al.  Negative priming , 2008, Scholarpedia.

[79]  A. Buchner,et al.  Negative Priming as a Memory Phenomenon , 2007 .

[80]  Dagmar Verbaan,et al.  Cognitive impairment in Parkinson's , 2007 .

[81]  J. Stout,et al.  Enhanced negative priming in Parkinson's disease. , 2002, Neuropsychology.

[82]  J. Filoteo,et al.  Divergent findings regarding negative priming in Parkinson's disease: A comment on Filoteo et al. (2002) and Wylie and Stout (2002). , 2002, Neuropsychology.

[83]  Riccardo Russo,et al.  Age differences and the identity negative priming effect: an updated meta-analysis. , 2002, Psychology and aging.

[84]  Dorit Wenke,et al.  Ambiguity resolution as a function of reading skill, age, dementia, and schizophrenia: The role of attentional control. , 2001 .

[85]  J. Nigg On inhibition/disinhibition in developmental psychopathology: views from cognitive and personality psychology and a working inhibition taxonomy. , 2000, Psychological bulletin.

[86]  B. Murdoch,et al.  Dopaminergic modulation of semantic activation: Evidence from Parkinson's disease , 2000 .

[87]  B. Snow,et al.  Criteria for diagnosing Parkinson's disease , 1992, Annals of neurology.

[88]  W. Neill,et al.  Persistence of negative priming: II. Evidence for episodic trace retrieval. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[89]  S. Tipper,et al.  Negative priming between pictures and words in a selective attention task: Evidence for semantic processing of ignored stimuli , 1988, Memory & cognition.

[90]  J. Morton The logogen model and orthographic structure , 1980 .