Low cognitive load strengthens distractor interference while high load attenuates when cognitive load and distractor possess similar visual characteristics
暂无分享,去创建一个
Naoyuki Osaka | Zach Shipstead | Randall W Engle | Takehiro Minamoto | R. Engle | T. Minamoto | N. Osaka | Zach Shipstead
[1] P. Lang. International affective picture system (IAPS) : affective ratings of pictures and instruction manual , 2005 .
[2] M. Osaka,et al. Incidental encoding of goal irrelevant information is associated with insufficient engagement of the dorsal frontal cortex and the inferior parietal cortex , 2012, Brain Research.
[3] Philip A. Kragel,et al. Regional brain differences in the effect of distraction during the delay interval of a working memory task , 2007, Brain Research.
[4] Triesch Jochen. Object perception is selectively slowed by a visually similar working memory load , 2008 .
[5] J. Theeuwes,et al. Feature-based memory-driven attentional capture: visual working memory content affects visual attention. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[6] Olivia S. Cheung,et al. Selective interference on the holistic processing of faces in working memory. , 2010, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[7] M. Osaka,et al. Individual differences in working memory capacity and distractor processing: Possible contribution of top–down inhibitory control , 2010, Brain Research.
[8] R. Engle,et al. The role of prefrontal cortex in working-memory capacity, executive attention, and general fluid intelligence: An individual-differences perspective , 2002, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[9] C. Eriksen,et al. Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task , 1974 .
[10] C. Frith,et al. The Role of Working Memory in Visual Selective Attention , 2001, Science.
[11] Iain D Gilchrist,et al. Oculomotor capture by transient events: a comparison of abrupt onsets, offsets, motion, and flicker. , 2008, Journal of vision.
[12] A. Anastasi. Individual differences. , 2020, Annual review of psychology.
[13] J. Theeuwes,et al. Attentional control during visual search: the effect of irrelevant singletons. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[14] G. Humphreys,et al. Stressing the mind: The effect of cognitive load and articulatory suppression on attentional guidance from working memory , 2008, Perception & psychophysics.
[15] R. K. Abolfazlian,et al. The Cocktail Party Listener , 1994 .
[16] C. Tredoux,et al. Recognition and context memory for faces from own and other ethnic groups: A remember-know investigation , 2010, Memory & cognition.
[17] Michael F. Bunting,et al. The cocktail party phenomenon revisited: The importance of working memory capacity , 2001, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[18] Marvin M Chun,et al. Concurrent working memory load can reduce distraction. , 2005, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[19] Richard P. Heitz,et al. Complex working memory span tasks and higher-order cognition: A latent-variable analysis of the relationship between processing and storage , 2009, Memory.
[20] R. Engle,et al. The nature of individual differences in working memory capacity: active maintenance in primary memory and controlled search from secondary memory. , 2007, Psychological review.
[21] Josef C. Schrock,et al. Working memory capacity and the antisaccade task: individual differences in voluntary saccade control. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[22] T. Klingberg,et al. Prefrontal cortex and basal ganglia control access to working memory , 2008, Nature Neuroscience.
[23] P. Carpenter,et al. Individual differences in working memory and reading , 1980 .
[24] Amishi P. Jha,et al. The role of prefrontal cortex in resolving distractor interference , 2004, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.
[25] Maro G. Machizawa,et al. Neural measures reveal individual differences in controlling access to working memory , 2005, Nature.
[26] T. Eliot. The Cocktail Party , 1980 .
[27] Matthew P. Gerrie,et al. Individual differences in working memory capacity and visual search: The roles of top-down and bottom-up processing , 2007, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[28] M. Chun,et al. Concurrent working memory load can facilitate selective attention: evidence for specialized load. , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[29] G. Humphreys,et al. Automatic guidance of attention from working memory , 2008, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[30] J. Theeuwes,et al. Faces capture attention: Evidence from inhibition of return , 2006 .
[31] P. Cavanagh,et al. The Capacity of Visual Short-Term Memory is Set Both by Visual Information Load and by Number of Objects , 2004, Psychological science.
[32] E. Viding,et al. Load theory of selective attention and cognitive control. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[33] David Soto,et al. Automatic guidance of visual attention from verbal working memory. , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[34] R. Engle,et al. Working-memory capacity and the control of attention: the contributions of goal neglect, response competition, and task set to Stroop interference. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[35] M. Just,et al. From the SelectedWorks of Marcel Adam Just 1992 A capacity theory of comprehension : Individual differences in working memory , 2017 .
[36] Richard P. Heitz,et al. An automated version of the operation span task , 2005, Behavior research methods.
[37] Wen Gao,et al. The CAS-PEAL Large-Scale Chinese Face Database and Baseline Evaluations , 2008, IEEE Transactions on Systems, Man, and Cybernetics - Part A: Systems and Humans.
[38] R. Malpass,et al. Recognition for faces of own and other race. , 1969, Journal of personality and social psychology.
[39] R. Engle,et al. Is working memory capacity task dependent , 1989 .