Polarity-dependent Effects of Biparietal Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation on the Interplay between Target Location and Distractor Saliency in Visual Attention

Visual attention allows the allocation of limited neural processing resources to stimuli based on their behavioral priorities. The selection of task-relevant visual targets entails the processing of multiple competing stimuli and the suppression of distractors that may be either perceptually salient or perceptually similar to targets. The posterior parietal cortex controls the interaction between top–down (task-driven) and bottom–up (stimulus-driven) processes competing for attentional selection, as well as spatial distribution of attention. Here, we examined whether biparietal transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) would modulate the interaction between top–down and bottom–up processes in visual attention. Visual attention function was assessed with a visual discrimination task, in which a lateralized target was presented alone or together with a contralateral, similar or salient, distractor. The accuracy and RTs were measured before and during three stimulation sessions (sham, right anodal/left cathodal, left anodal/right cathodal). The analyses demonstrated (i) polarity-dependent effects of tDCS on the accuracy of target discrimination, but only when the target was presented with a similar distractor; (ii) the tDCS-triggered effects on the accuracy of discriminating targets, accompanied by a similar distractor, varied according to the target location; and (iii) overall detrimental effects of tDCS on RTs were observed, regardless of target location, distractor type, and polarity of the stimulation. We conclude that the observed polarity, distractor type, and target location-dependent effects of biparietal tDCS on the accuracy of target detection resulted from both a modulation of the interaction between top–down and bottom–up attentional processes and the interhemispheric competition mechanisms guiding attentional selection and spatial deployment of attention.

[1]  J. Mattingley,et al.  Dissociable effects of anodal and cathodal tDCS reveal distinct functional roles for right parietal cortex in the detection of single and competing stimuli , 2015, Neuropsychologia.

[2]  J. Marshall,et al.  Spatial cognition: evidence from visual neglect , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[3]  David J. Sharp,et al.  The effect of oppositional parietal transcranial direct current stimulation on lateralized brain functions , 2015, The European journal of neuroscience.

[4]  Joy J. Geng,et al.  Attentional Mechanisms of Distractor Suppression , 2014 .

[5]  Howard E Egeth,et al.  Biased competition and visual search: the role of luminance and size contrast , 2007, Psychological research.

[6]  Jeffrey N. Rouder,et al.  Bayesian inference for psychology. Part II: Example applications with JASP , 2017, Psychonomic Bulletin & Review.

[7]  G. Fink,et al.  Modulation of Top-Down Control of Visual Attention by Cathodal tDCS over Right IPS , 2012, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[8]  Michael D. Rugg,et al.  Cognitive functions of the posterior parietal cortex , 2013, Front. Integr. Neurosci..

[9]  H. Müller,et al.  Salience-Based Selection: Attentional Capture by Distractors Less Salient Than the Target , 2013, PloS one.

[10]  Carmel Mevorach,et al.  Driven to less distraction: rTMS of the right parietal cortex reduces attentional capture in visual search. , 2009, Cerebral cortex.

[11]  Eric Maris,et al.  Attentional Cues Affect Accuracy and Reaction Time via Different Cognitive and Neural Processes , 2012, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[12]  H. Jasper,et al.  The ten-twenty electrode system of the International Federation. The International Federation of Clinical Neurophysiology. , 1999, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology. Supplement.

[13]  Carmel Mevorach,et al.  Opposite biases in salience-based selection for the left and right posterior parietal cortex , 2006, Nature Neuroscience.

[14]  Joy J Geng,et al.  Attentional capture by a perceptually salient non-target facilitates target processing through inhibition and rapid rejection. , 2010, Journal of vision.

[15]  Gereon R. Fink,et al.  Cue validity modulates the neural correlates of covert endogenous orienting of attention in parietal and frontal cortex , 2006, NeuroImage.

[16]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Human cortical mechanisms of visual attention during orienting and search. , 1998, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[17]  L. M. M.-T. Theory of Probability , 1929, Nature.

[18]  Ali Mazaheri,et al.  Pre-Stimulus Activity Predicts the Winner of Top-Down vs. Bottom-Up Attentional Selection , 2011, PloS one.

[19]  R. C. Oldfield The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. , 1971, Neuropsychologia.

[20]  G. Orban,et al.  The influence of stimulus location on the brain activation pattern in detection and orientation discrimination. A PET study of visual attention. , 1996, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[21]  L. Cohen,et al.  Transcranial direct current stimulation: State of the art 2008 , 2008, Brain Stimulation.

[22]  K. Heilman,et al.  Pseudoneglect: Effects of hemispace on a tactile line bisection task , 1980, Neuropsychologia.

[23]  C. Miniussi,et al.  The Role of Timing in the Induction of Neuromodulation in Perceptual Learning by Transcranial Electric Stimulation , 2013, Brain Stimulation.

[24]  M. Corbetta,et al.  A Common Network of Functional Areas for Attention and Eye Movements , 1998, Neuron.

[25]  M. Corbetta,et al.  An Event-Related Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Study of Voluntary and Stimulus-Driven Orienting of Attention , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[26]  B. Fierro,et al.  Neglect-like effects induced by tDCS modulation of posterior parietal cortices in healthy subjects , 2011, Brain Stimulation.

[27]  Hermann J. Müller,et al.  Sources of Top–Down Control in Visual Search , 2009, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[28]  M. Kinsbourne Mechanisms of Unilateral Neglect , 1987 .

[29]  L. B. Roy,et al.  Modulation of attention functions by anodal tDCS on right PPC , 2015, Neuropsychologia.

[30]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Right Hemisphere Dominance during Spatial Selective Attention and Target Detection Occurs Outside the Dorsal Frontoparietal Network , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[31]  G. Vallar Spatial hemineglect in humans , 1998, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[32]  J. Theeuwes,et al.  Electrophysiological Evidence of the Capture of Visual Attention , 2006, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[33]  Jeffrey N. Rouder,et al.  Bayesian inference for psychology. Part I: Theoretical advantages and practical ramifications , 2017, Psychonomic Bulletin & Review.

[34]  G. Fink,et al.  Dorsal and Ventral Attention Systems : Distinct Neural Circuits but Collaborative Roles , 2013 .

[35]  M. McCourt,et al.  Pseudoneglect: a review and meta-analysis of performance factors in line bisection tasks , 2000, Neuropsychologia.

[36]  Michal Lavidor,et al.  When Less Is More: Evidence for a Facilitative Cathodal tDCS Effect in Attentional Abilities , 2012, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[37]  Joy J. Geng,et al.  Contextual Knowledge Configures Attentional Control Networks , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[38]  B. Krekelberg,et al.  Transcranial direct current stimulation over posterior parietal cortex modulates visuospatial localization. , 2014, Journal of vision.

[39]  J. Wolfe,et al.  What attributes guide the deployment of visual attention and how do they do it? , 2004, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[40]  Jeffrey N. Rouder,et al.  Bayesian t tests for accepting and rejecting the null hypothesis , 2009, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[41]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Neural Systems for Visual Orienting and Their Relationships to Spatial Working Memory , 2002, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[42]  Pierre Jolicoeur,et al.  Tracking the Location of Visuospatial Attention in a Contingent Capture Paradigm , 2008, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[43]  A. Antal,et al.  Safety aspects of transcranial direct current stimulation concerning healthy subjects and patients , 2007, Brain Research Bulletin.

[44]  George R. Mangun,et al.  Right temporoparietal junction activation by a salient contextual cue facilitates target discrimination , 2011, NeuroImage.

[45]  M. Nitsche,et al.  Excitability changes induced in the human motor cortex by weak transcranial direct current stimulation , 2000, The Journal of physiology.

[46]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain , 2002, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[47]  Glyn W. Humphreys,et al.  The Left Intraparietal Sulcus Modulates the Selection of Low Salient Stimuli , 2009, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[48]  N. Bolognini,et al.  Brain polarization of parietal cortex augments training-induced improvement of visual exploratory and attentional skills , 2010, Brain Research.

[49]  Marcel Kinsbourne,et al.  Orientational bias model of unilateral neglect: Evidence from attentional gradients within hemispace. , 2013 .

[50]  Abhishek Datta,et al.  Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation Accelerates Allocentric Target Detection , 2013, Brain Stimulation.

[51]  M. Lee,et al.  Statistical Evidence in Experimental Psychology , 2011, Perspectives on psychological science : a journal of the Association for Psychological Science.

[52]  Joy J. Geng,et al.  Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews Review Re-evaluating the Role of Tpj in Attentional Control: Contextual Updating? , 2022 .

[53]  Richard S. J. Frackowiak,et al.  Functional localization of the system for visuospatial attention using positron emission tomography. , 1997, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[54]  G. Fink,et al.  Bidirectional alterations of interhemispheric parietal balance by non-invasive cortical stimulation. , 2009, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[55]  H. Egeth,et al.  Target-nontarget similarity modulates stimulus-driven control in visual search , 2006, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[56]  K. Heilman,et al.  Mechanisms underlying hemispatial neglect , 1979, Annals of neurology.

[57]  C. Bundesen A theory of visual attention. , 1990, Psychological review.

[58]  C. Miniussi,et al.  Non-linear effects of transcranial direct current stimulation as a function of individual baseline performance: Evidence from biparietal tDCS influence on lateralized attention bias , 2015, Cortex.

[59]  William Prinzmetal,et al.  Attention: reaction time and accuracy reveal different mechanisms. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[60]  K. Heilman,et al.  Right hemisphere dominance for attention , 1980, Neurology.

[61]  J. Theeuwes Perceptual selectivity for color and form , 1992, Perception & psychophysics.

[62]  Sarah Shomstein,et al.  Cognitive functions of the posterior parietal cortex: top-down and bottom-up attentional control , 2012, Front. Integr. Neurosci..

[63]  Susan L. Franzel,et al.  Guided search: an alternative to the feature integration model for visual search. , 1989, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[64]  M. Nicholls,et al.  Free-viewing perceptual asymmetries for the judgement of brightness, numerosity and size , 1999, Neuropsychologia.

[65]  M. Nitsche,et al.  Physiological Basis of Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation , 2011, The Neuroscientist : a review journal bringing neurobiology, neurology and psychiatry.

[66]  M. Corbetta,et al.  The Reorienting System of the Human Brain: From Environment to Theory of Mind , 2008, Neuron.

[67]  S. Yantis,et al.  On the distinction between visual salience and stimulus-driven attentional capture. , 1999, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[68]  Heidi Johansen-Berg,et al.  Predicting behavioural response to TDCS in chronic motor stroke☆ , 2014, NeuroImage.

[69]  M. Hallett,et al.  What does the ratio of injected current to electrode area tell us about current density in the brain during tDCS? , 2009, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[70]  Gottfried Schlaug,et al.  Anodal Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation of the Prefrontal Cortex Enhances Complex Verbal Associative Thought , 2009, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[71]  Leendert van Maanen,et al.  UvA-DARE ( Digital Academic Repository ) Are accuracy and reaction time affected via different processes ? , 2013 .

[72]  E. Macaluso,et al.  Neural correlates of the spatial and expectancy components of endogenous and stimulus-driven orienting of attention in the Posner task. , 2010, Cerebral cortex.

[73]  M. Mesulam A cortical network for directed attention and unilateral neglect , 1981, Annals of neurology.

[74]  M. Husain,et al.  The functional role of the inferior parietal lobe in the dorsal and ventral stream dichotomy , 2009, Neuropsychologia.

[75]  Justin L. Vincent,et al.  Spontaneous neuronal activity distinguishes human dorsal and ventral attention systems. , 2006, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[76]  M. Nitsche,et al.  Sustained excitability elevations induced by transcranial DC motor cortex stimulation in humans , 2001, Neurology.

[77]  Felipe Fregni,et al.  Visual memory improved by non-invasive brain stimulation , 2010, Brain Research.

[78]  Mark E. McCourt,et al.  Visuospatial attention in line bisection: stimulusmodulation of pseudoneglect , 1999, Neuropsychologia.

[79]  Carmel Mevorach,et al.  Ignoring the Elephant in the Room: A Neural Circuit to Downregulate Salience , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[80]  Felix M Mottaghy,et al.  Noninvasive brain stimulation with transcranial magnetic or direct current stimulation (TMS/tDCS)-From insights into human memory to therapy of its dysfunction. , 2008, Methods.

[81]  S. Yantis 3 Goal-Directed and Stimulus-Driven Determinants of Attentional Control , 2000 .

[82]  F. J. Friedrich,et al.  Effects of parietal injury on covert orienting of attention , 1984, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[83]  J. Mattingley,et al.  Parietal neglect and visual awareness , 1998, Nature Neuroscience.

[84]  J. Theeuwes Top-down and bottom-up control of visual selection. , 2010, Acta psychologica.

[85]  J. Duncan The locus of interference in the perception of simultaneous stimuli. , 1980, Psychological review.

[86]  J. Duncan,et al.  Visual search and stimulus similarity. , 1989, Psychological review.

[87]  Glyn W. Humphreys,et al.  Effects of saliency, not global dominance, in patients with left parietal damage , 2006, Neuropsychologia.

[88]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Spatial neglect and attention networks. , 2011, Annual review of neuroscience.

[89]  Steven Yantis,et al.  Stimulus-Driven and Goal-Directed Attentional Control , 2002 .

[90]  U. Neisser VISUAL SEARCH. , 1964, Scientific American.

[91]  F. J. Friedrich,et al.  Spatial attention deficits in humans: a comparison of superior parietal and temporal-parietal junction lesions. , 1998, Neuropsychology.

[92]  Paul B. Fitzgerald,et al.  Investigating the Role of Current Strength in tDCS Modulation of Working Memory Performance in Healthy Controls , 2011, Front. Psychiatry.