Posterior Medial Frontal Cortex Activity Predicts Post-Error Adaptations in Task-Related Visual and Motor Areas

As Seneca the Younger put it, “To err is human, but to persist is diabolical.” To prevent repetition of errors, human performance monitoring often triggers adaptations such as general slowing and/or attentional focusing. The posterior medial frontal cortex (pMFC) is assumed to monitor performance problems and to interact with other brain areas that implement the necessary adaptations. Whereas previous research showed interactions between pMFC and lateral-prefrontal regions, here we demonstrate that upon the occurrence of errors the pMFC selectively interacts with perceptual and motor regions and thereby drives attentional focusing toward task-relevant information and induces motor adaptation observed as post-error slowing. Functional magnetic resonance imaging data from an interference task reveal that error-related pMFC activity predicts the following: (1) subsequent activity enhancement in perceptual areas encoding task-relevant stimulus features; (2) activity suppression in perceptual areas encoding distracting stimulus features; and (3) post-error slowing-related activity decrease in the motor system. Additionally, diffusion-weighted imaging revealed a correlation of individual post-error slowing and white matter integrity beneath pMFC regions that are connected to the motor inhibition system, encompassing right inferior frontal gyrus and subthalamic nucleus. Thus, disturbances in task performance are remedied by functional interactions of the pMFC with multiple task-related brain regions beyond prefrontal cortex that result in a broad repertoire of adaptive processes at perceptual as well as motor levels.

[1]  Wim Fias,et al.  Post-error slowing: An orienting account , 2009, Cognition.

[2]  J. Pekar,et al.  A method for making group inferences from functional MRI data using independent component analysis , 2001, Human brain mapping.

[3]  Elisa Ciaramelli,et al.  The Regulation of Cognitive Control following Rostral Anterior Cingulate Cortex Lesion in Humans , 2007, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[4]  S. Treue Neural correlates of attention in primate visual cortex , 2001, Trends in Neurosciences.

[5]  Ethan R. Buch,et al.  Cortical and subcortical interactions during action reprogramming and their related white matter pathways , 2010, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[6]  Mark D'Esposito,et al.  Searching for “the Top” in Top-Down Control , 2005, Neuron.

[7]  E. Koechlin,et al.  Motivation and cognitive control in the human prefrontal cortex , 2009, Nature Neuroscience.

[8]  Timothy E. J. Behrens,et al.  Optimal decision making and the anterior cingulate cortex , 2006, Nature Neuroscience.

[9]  Joaquín M. Fuster,et al.  Executive frontal functions , 2000, Experimental Brain Research.

[10]  Mark W. Woolrich,et al.  Probabilistic diffusion tractography with multiple fibre orientations: What can we gain? , 2007, NeuroImage.

[11]  Anna C Nobre,et al.  Subsecond Changes in Top–Down Control Exerted by Human Medial Frontal Cortex during Conflict and Action Selection: A Combined Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation–Electroencephalography Study , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[12]  M. D’Esposito,et al.  The variability of human BOLD hemodynamic responses , 1998, NeuroImage.

[13]  P. Rabbitt Errors and error correction in choice-response tasks. , 1966, Journal of experimental psychology.

[14]  C. Carter,et al.  Anterior cingulate cortex and conflict detection: An update of theory and data , 2007, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[15]  Josep Marco-Pallarés,et al.  Neural Mechanisms Underlying Adaptive Actions after Slips , 2008, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[16]  Stephen M. Smith,et al.  Temporal Autocorrelation in Univariate Linear Modeling of FMRI Data , 2001, NeuroImage.

[17]  C. Wehrhahn,et al.  Evidence for the contribution of S cones to the detection of flicker brightness and red-green. , 2000, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, image science, and vision.

[18]  W. Gehring,et al.  More attention must be paid: The neurobiology of attentional effort , 2006, Brain Research Reviews.

[19]  Michael Brady,et al.  Improved Optimization for the Robust and Accurate Linear Registration and Motion Correction of Brain Images , 2002, NeuroImage.

[20]  K. R. Ridderinkhof,et al.  Striatum and pre-SMA facilitate decision-making under time pressure , 2008, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[21]  Mark W. Woolrich,et al.  Advances in functional and structural MR image analysis and implementation as FSL , 2004, NeuroImage.

[22]  K. R. Ridderinkhof,et al.  The Role of the Medial Frontal Cortex in Cognitive Control , 2004, Science.

[23]  P. Basser Diffusion MRI: From Quantitative Measurement to In vivo Neuroanatomy , 2009 .

[24]  Etienne Olivier,et al.  Short-Latency Influence of Medial Frontal Cortex on Primary Motor Cortex during Action Selection under Conflict , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[25]  H. Garavan,et al.  Dissociable Executive Functions in the Dynamic Control of Behavior: Inhibition, Error Detection, and Correction , 2002, NeuroImage.

[26]  Hermann J. Müller,et al.  Selective and interactive neural correlates of visual dimension changes and response changes , 2006, NeuroImage.

[27]  Daniel Rueckert,et al.  Tract-based spatial statistics: Voxelwise analysis of multi-subject diffusion data , 2006, NeuroImage.

[28]  Aapo Hyvärinen,et al.  Validating the independent components of neuroimaging time series via clustering and visualization , 2004, NeuroImage.

[29]  Stephen M Smith,et al.  Fast robust automated brain extraction , 2002, Human brain mapping.

[30]  Norbert Kathmann,et al.  Neural correlates of error awareness , 2007, NeuroImage.

[31]  Stephen M. Smith,et al.  A global optimisation method for robust affine registration of brain images , 2001, Medical Image Anal..

[32]  D Yves von Cramon,et al.  Interactions of focal cortical lesions with error processing: evidence from event-related brain potentials. , 2002, Neuropsychology.

[33]  D. Meyer,et al.  A Neural System for Error Detection and Compensation , 1993 .

[34]  Mark D'Esposito,et al.  Variation of BOLD hemodynamic responses across subjects and brain regions and their effects on statistical analyses , 2004, NeuroImage.

[35]  K. R. Ridderinkhof,et al.  Response inhibition in conflict tasks is revealed in delta plots , 2004 .

[36]  M. Botvinick,et al.  Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. , 2001, Psychological review.

[37]  C. Beaulieu The Biological Basis of Diffusion Anisotropy , 2009 .

[38]  T. Egner,et al.  Cognitive control mechanisms resolve conflict through cortical amplification of task-relevant information , 2005, Nature Neuroscience.

[39]  H J Müller,et al.  Top-down controlled visual dimension weighting: an event-related fMRI study. , 2002, Cerebral cortex.

[40]  G A Orban,et al.  Attention-dependent suppression of metabolic activity in the early stages of the macaque visual system. , 2000, Cerebral cortex.

[41]  Kenneth Hugdahl,et al.  Prediction of human errors by maladaptive changes in event-related brain networks , 2008, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[42]  Thomas E. Nichols,et al.  Acquisition and voxelwise analysis of multi-subject diffusion data with Tract-Based Spatial Statistics , 2007, Nature Protocols.

[43]  John J. B. Allen,et al.  Prelude to and Resolution of an Error: EEG Phase Synchrony Reveals Cognitive Control Dynamics during Action Monitoring , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[44]  A. Kleinschmidt,et al.  Cross-Modal Processing in Early Visual and Auditory Cortices depends on Expected Statistical Relationship of Multisensory Information , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[45]  Xiangyu Long,et al.  Functional segmentation of the brain cortex using high model order group PICA , 2009, Human brain mapping.

[46]  E. Miller,et al.  An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. , 2001, Annual review of neuroscience.

[47]  D. Yves von Cramon,et al.  The Role of Intact Frontostriatal Circuits in Error Processing , 2006, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[48]  D. V. Cramon,et al.  Subprocesses of Performance Monitoring: A Dissociation of Error Processing and Response Competition Revealed by Event-Related fMRI and ERPs , 2001, NeuroImage.

[49]  C. Liston,et al.  Anterior Cingulate and Posterior Parietal Cortices Are Sensitive to Dissociable Forms of Conflict in a Task-Switching Paradigm , 2006, Neuron.

[50]  J. R. Simon,et al.  Reactions toward the source of stimulation. , 1969, Journal of experimental psychology.

[51]  C. Summerfield,et al.  Two Mechanisms for Task Switching in the Prefrontal Cortex , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[52]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Anterior Cingulate Conflict Monitoring and Adjustments in Control , 2004, Science.

[53]  A. Engel,et al.  Trial-by-Trial Coupling of Concurrent Electroencephalogram and Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Identifies the Dynamics of Performance Monitoring , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[54]  S G Lisberger,et al.  Neuronal responses in visual areas MT and MST during smooth pursuit target selection. , 1997, Journal of neurophysiology.

[55]  Franziska M. Korb,et al.  Post-Error Behavioral Adjustments Are Facilitated by Activation and Suppression of Task-Relevant and Task-Irrelevant Information Processing , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[56]  Notger G. Müller,et al.  The attentional ‘spotlight's’ penumbra: center-surround modulation in striate cortex , 2004, Neuroreport.

[57]  R. Knight,et al.  Prefrontal–cingulate interactions in action monitoring , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[58]  Timothy Edward John Behrens,et al.  Triangulating a Cognitive Control Network Using Diffusion-Weighted Magnetic Resonance Imaging (MRI) and Functional MRI , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[59]  John J. Foxe,et al.  Jumping the gun: is effective preparation contingent upon anticipatory activation in task-relevant neural circuitry? , 2006, Cerebral cortex.