Spatial Awareness: A Function of the Posterior Parietal Lobe?

Chronic unilateral visuo-spatial neglect is usually ‘defined’ clinically as a condition in which patients ignore objects and events in contralesional space. As such, ‘it’ has been observed after lesion to many distinct right hemisphere loci (and much more rarely after left hemisphere lesion). After cortico-subcortical damage, left neglect can be consequent upon right posterior parietal (Vallar and Perani, 1986), right temporal (Samuelsson et al., 1997), and right frontal (e.g., Damasio et al., 1980; Husain and Kennard, 1996) lesions. Neglect has also been reported after subcortical lesions of the thalamus (e.g., Watson and Heilman, 1979; Rafal and Posner, 1987), the basal ganglia (e.g., Damasio et al., 1980; Ferro et al., 1987) and the white matter/internal capsule (e.g., Healton et al., 1982; Ferro and Kertesz, 1984). There is, nonetheless, reasonable consensus that severe visuo-spatial neglect is most typically associated with lesion of the supramarginal gyrus of the right inferior parietal lobule – Brodmann’s area 39/40 (Vallar, 2001). Consensus is, of course, no guarantee of truth and some students have even argued that the detailed manifestations of so-called visuospatial neglect are too variable (both withinand between-patients) to be meaningfully united into one clinical entity (Halligan and Marshall, 1992; Stone et al., 1998). A recent report by Karnath et al. (2001) confirms that unilateral visuo-spatial neglect (in patients who meet the customary clinical features thereof) can result from lesions of right temporal cortex and from lesions of right parietal cortex (Vallar, 2001). But, more controversially, Karnath et al. then make a partially novel functional and anatomical distinction between two types of spatial neglect. In “pure” spatial neglect there is purportedly loss of “spatial awareness” contralateral to the lesion; the locus of the responsible lesion is exclusively superior temporal, without extension into occipital cortex or the optic radiations (and hence the patients do not manifest visual field deficits). By contrast, in ‘impure’ spatial neglect, there is a disorder of “directly coding space for action” (but not apparently of spatial awareness); here the lesion includes the inferiorposterior parietal cortex but also extends into regions which, when damaged, produce visual field deficits (see also Karnath, 2001, for further discussion). The functional distinction that Karnath et al. (2001) draw has some similarities with the contrast that Poppelreuter (1917) made between the roles of attentional

[1]  A. Milner,et al.  An Investigation of Hemispatial Neglect Using the Landmark Task , 1995, Brain and Cognition.

[2]  Hans Samuelsson,et al.  Anatomical and Neurological Correlates of Acute and Chronic Visuospatial Neglect Following Right Hemisphere Stroke * * Some of the results of this study were presented at: The 5th Nordic Meeting in Neuropsychology, Uppsala, Sweden, August 17-19, 1995. , 1997, Cortex.

[3]  Edoardo Bisiach,et al.  Perceptual and Response Bias in Unilateral Neglect: Two Modified Versions of the Milner Landmark Task , 1998, Brain and Cognition.

[4]  R. Logie,et al.  What the Eyes Perceive, The Brain Ignores: A Case of Pure Unilateral Representational Neglect , 1997, Cortex.

[5]  Christopher Kennard,et al.  Visual neglect associated with frontal lobe infarction , 1996, Journal of Neurology.

[6]  G. Vallar Extrapersonal Visual Unilateral Spatial Neglect and Its Neuroanatomy , 2001, NeuroImage.

[7]  M. Posner,et al.  Deficits in human visual spatial attention following thalamic lesions. , 1987, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[8]  F. Doricchi,et al.  Misrepresentation of horizontal space in left unilateral neglect , 1999, Neurology.

[9]  J. Ferro,et al.  Subcortical neglect , 1987, Neurology.

[10]  M D'Esposito,et al.  Assessment of neglect reveals dissociable behavioral but not neuroanatomical subtypes , 1996, Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society.

[11]  G. Vallar Left spatial hemineglect: An unmanageable explosion of dissociations? no , 1994 .

[12]  J. Marshall,et al.  VISUOSPATIAL NEGLECT: UNDERLYING FACTORS AND TEST SENSITIVITY , 1989, The Lancet.

[13]  Edward B. Healton Subcortical neglect , 1984, Neurology.

[14]  Michael Fetter,et al.  Ocular space exploration in the dark and its relation to subjective and objective body orientation in neglect patients with parietal lesions , 1995, Neuropsychologia.

[15]  Hanna Damasio,et al.  Neglect following damage to frontal lobe or basal ganglia , 1980, Neuropsychologia.

[16]  D. Perani,et al.  The anatomy of unilateral neglect after right-hemisphere stroke lesions. A clinical/CT-scan correlation study in man , 1986, Neuropsychologia.

[17]  J. Duhamel,et al.  Neglect in Monkeys : effect of permanent and reversible lesions. , 2002 .

[18]  J. Marshall,et al.  Unilateral neglect , 1998, Neurology.

[19]  C. Kennard,et al.  Impaired spatial working memory across saccades contributes to abnormal search in parietal neglect. , 2001, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[20]  S. Ferber,et al.  Spatial awareness is a function of the temporal not the posterior parietal lobe , 2001, Nature.

[21]  John C. Marshall,et al.  Left Visuo-Spatial Neglect: A Meaningless Entity? , 1992, Cortex.

[22]  Claus C. Hilgetag,et al.  Models of paradoxical lesion effects and rules of inference for imputing function to structure in the brain , 1999, Neurocomputing.

[23]  John C. Marshall,et al.  Seeing the forest but only half the trees? , 1995, Nature.

[24]  H. Karnath New insights into the functions of the superior temporal cortex , 2001, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[25]  K M Heilman,et al.  Directional hypokinesia and hemispatial inattention in neglect. , 1990, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[26]  L. Buxbaum,et al.  Neglect of chimeric figures: Two halves are better than a whole , 1994, Neuropsychologia.

[27]  P. Halligan,et al.  Measuring visual neglect in acute stroke and predicting its recovery: the visual neglect recovery index. , 1992, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[28]  J. Ferro,et al.  Posterior internal capsule infarction associated with neglect. , 1984, Archives of neurology.

[29]  Giacomo Rizzolatti,et al.  Visual Processing without Awareness: Evidence from Unilateral Neglect , 1992, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[30]  Filippo Brighina,et al.  Contralateral neglect induced by right posterior parietal rTMS in healthy subjects , 2000, Neuroreport.

[31]  Gereon R. Fink,et al.  Spatial Cognition: Where We Were and Where We Are , 2001, NeuroImage.

[32]  R. Sterzi,et al.  Directional hypokinesia in spatial hemineglect: a case study. , 1992, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[33]  Manjit,et al.  Neurology , 1912, NeuroImage.

[34]  K. Zilles,et al.  Line bisection judgments implicate right parietal cortex and cerebellum as assessed by fMRI , 2000, Neurology.

[35]  K. Heilman,et al.  Neglect and Related Disorders , 1984, Seminars in neurology.

[36]  K. Heilman,et al.  Thalamic neglect , 1979, Neurology.

[37]  P. Astikainen,et al.  Cortical and subcortical visual event‐related potentials to oddball stimuli in rabbits , 2000, Neuroreport.

[38]  D L Na,et al.  Dissociation of sensory-attentional from motor-intentional neglect , 1998, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.