Impulsive responding on the peak-interval procedure

The pattern of responding on a peak-interval timing task allows one to make inferences regarding the sources of variation that contribute to interval timing behavior. Non-temporal factors such as impulsivity may impact the validity of these inferences. Rats were trained on a 15s peak-interval procedure (PI) or a mixed 15s behaviorally dependent variable-interval, 15s peak-interval procedure (bdVIPI) for an extended number of sessions. Extended training on the PI revealed a bi-modal distribution in the times at which subjects started responding for temporally predictable reinforcement, suggesting that multiple processes contribute to the behavioral pattern obtained in this procedure. Training on the bdVIPI eliminated the early mode of this bi-modal distribution, thereby decreasing the variation in start times. These results suggest that alternative response options can modulate the influence of impulsivity in timing tasks.

[1]  Catalin V. Buhusi,et al.  What makes us tick? Functional and neural mechanisms of interval timing , 2005, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[2]  R. Church,et al.  Representation of time , 1990, Cognition.

[3]  Temporal integration as a function of signal and gap intensity in rats (Rattus norvegicus) and pigeons (Columba livia). , 2002 .

[4]  C. M. Bradshaw,et al.  Evidence that the effect of 5-HT2 receptor stimulation on temporal differentiation is not mediated by receptors in the dorsal striatum , 2006, Behavioural Processes.

[5]  T. Robbins,et al.  Increased response switching, perseveration and perseverative switching following d-amphetamine in the rat , 2004, Psychopharmacology.

[6]  Armando Machado,et al.  Acquisition and extinction under periodic reinforcement , 1998, Behavioural Processes.

[7]  N. McNaughton,et al.  Common Firing Patterns of Hippocampal Cells in a Differential Reinforcement of Low Rates of Response Schedule , 2000, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[8]  R. Cheng,et al.  Acute effects of d-amphetamine on the differential reinforcement of low-rate (DRL) schedule behavior in the rat: comparison with selective dopamine receptor antagonists. , 2005, The Chinese journal of physiology.

[9]  The effects of selected antidepressant drugs on timing behaviour in rats , 1998, Psychopharmacology.

[10]  J. Evenden The pharmacology of impulsive behaviour in rats IV: the effects of selective serotonergic agents on a paced fixed consecutive number schedule , 1998, Psychopharmacology.

[11]  M. Ho,et al.  5-Hydroxytryptamine and interval timing behaviour , 2002, Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior.

[12]  S. Mobini,et al.  Effect of 8-OH-DPAT on temporal discrimination following central 5-hydroxytryptamine depletion , 2002, Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior.

[13]  W. Meck Functional and neural mechanisms of interval timing , 2003 .

[14]  James G. Herndon,et al.  Anatomical specificity within rat striatum for the dopaminergic modulation of DRL responding and activity , 1978, Brain Research.

[15]  J. Gibbon Scalar expectancy theory and Weber's law in animal timing. , 1977 .

[16]  R. Church,et al.  Application of scalar timing theory to individual trials. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[17]  C. M. Bradshaw,et al.  Time and behaviour : psychological and neurobehavioural analyses , 1997 .

[18]  A. Nieoullon,et al.  Complex Deficits on Reaction Time Performance following Bilateral Intrastriatal 6‐OHDA Infusion in the Rat , 1995, The European journal of neuroscience.

[19]  M. Ho,et al.  Effects of fenfluramine on free-operant timing behaviour: evidence for involvement of 5-HT2A receptors , 2004, Psychopharmacology.

[20]  S. Mobini,et al.  Antagonism by WAY-100635 of the effects of 8-OH-DPAT on performance on a free-operant timing schedule in intact and 5-HT-depleted rats , 2002, Behavioural pharmacology.

[21]  J. Evenden The effects of psychotomimetics and psychomotor stimulants on two schedules promoting response switching in the rat , 2002, Psychopharmacology.

[22]  Melissa Bateson,et al.  Interval Timing and Optimal Foraging , 2003 .

[23]  M. Bateson,et al.  Single-trials analyses demonstrate that increases in clock speed contribute to the methamphetamine-induced horizontal shifts in peak-interval timing functions , 2006, Psychopharmacology.

[24]  K. Cheng The form of timing distributions in pigeons under penalties for responding early , 1992 .

[25]  Catalin V Buhusi,et al.  Temporal integration as a function of signal and gap intensity in rats (Rattus norvegicus) and pigeons (Columba livia). , 2002, Journal of comparative psychology.

[26]  Russell M. Church,et al.  A connectionist model of timing. , 1991 .

[27]  G. Thines,et al.  Behavioural processes , 1976, Behavioural Processes.

[28]  J. Crystal,et al.  Simultaneous temporal and spatial processing , 2002, Animal learning & behavior.

[29]  R M Church,et al.  Comparison of variance and covariance patterns in parallel and serial theories of timing. , 1992, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[30]  J. Gibbon,et al.  Scalar expectancy theory and peak-interval timing in humans. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[31]  C. Bradshaw,et al.  Effects of 5-HT1A and 5-HT2A receptor stimulation on temporal differentiation performance in the fixed-interval peak procedure , 2006, Behavioural Processes.

[32]  Warren H. Meck,et al.  Symmetrical and asymmetrical sources of variance in temporal generalization , 1991 .

[33]  J. Evenden The pharmacology of impulsive behaviour in rats III: the effects of amphetamine, haloperidol, imipramine, chlordiazepoxide and ethanol on a paced fixed consecutive number schedule , 1998, Psychopharmacology.

[34]  J. H. Daruna,et al.  A neurodevelopmental view of impulsivity. , 1993 .

[35]  R. Church,et al.  Simultaneous temporal processing. , 1984, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[36]  W. Meck,et al.  Neuropsychological mechanisms of interval timing behavior. , 2000, BioEssays : news and reviews in molecular, cellular and developmental biology.

[37]  J. Evenden,et al.  The psychopharmacology of impulsive behaviour in rats VIII: effects of amphetamine, methylphenidate, and other drugs on responding maintained by a fixed consecutive number avoidance schedule , 2005, Psychopharmacology.

[38]  C. Gallistel,et al.  Sources of variability and systematic error in mouse timing behavior. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[39]  C. Bradshaw,et al.  Effects of quipazine and m-chlorophenylbiguanide (m-CPBG) on the discrimination of durations: evidence for the involvement of 5-HT2A but not 5-HT3 receptors , 2005, Behavioural pharmacology.

[40]  J. Glennon,et al.  Effects of 5-HT2A receptor stimulation on the discrimination of durations by rats , 2006, Behavioural pharmacology.

[41]  M. Ho,et al.  Effect of central 5-hydroxytryptamine depletion on tolerance of delay of reinforcement: evidence from performance in a discrete-trials “time-left” procedure , 1999, Psychopharmacology.

[42]  J. Staddon,et al.  Time and memory: towards a pacemaker-free theory of interval timing. , 1999, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[43]  J. Evenden Varieties of impulsivity , 1999, Psychopharmacology.

[44]  Alex Kacelnik,et al.  Optimal foraging and timing processes in the starling, Sturnus vulgaris: effect of inter-capture interval , 1992, Animal Behaviour.

[45]  Myrna B. Shure,et al.  The Impulsive client : theory, research, and treatment , 1993 .

[46]  W. Meck Neuropharmacology of timing and time perception. , 1996, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[47]  Masataka Watanabe,et al.  Prefrontal and cingulate unit activity during timing behavior in the monkey , 1979, Brain Research.

[48]  J. Evenden The pharmacology of impulsive behaviour in rats II: the effects of amphetamine, haloperidol, imipramine, chlordiazepoxide and other drugs on fixed consecutive number schedules (FCN 8 and FCN 32) , 1998, Psychopharmacology.

[49]  W. Meck,et al.  Cortico-striatal circuits and interval timing: coincidence detection of oscillatory processes. , 2004, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[50]  Stephen Grossberg,et al.  Neural dynamics of adaptive timing and temporal discrimination during associative learning , 1989, Neural Networks.

[51]  Catalin V Buhusi,et al.  Memory for timing visual and auditory signals in albino and pigmented rats. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[52]  K. Cheng,et al.  Analysis of single trials in pigeons' timing performance. , 1993 .

[53]  P. Couvillon,et al.  Temporal control of conditioned responding in goldfish. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[54]  R M Church,et al.  Scalar Timing in Memory , 1984, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[55]  M. Ho,et al.  Effect of central 5-hydroxytryptamine depletion on performance in the “time-left” procedure: further evidence for a role of the 5-hydroxytryptaminergic pathways in behavioural “switching” , 1997, Psychopharmacology.

[56]  K. Cheng,et al.  Memory Variance in the Peak Procedure of Timing in Pigeons , 1993 .

[57]  P. Killeen,et al.  A behavioral theory of timing. , 1988, Psychological review.

[58]  Effects of Restraint on Temporal Discrimination Behavior , 1974 .

[59]  W. N. Schoenfeld The Theory of reinforcement schedules , 1970 .

[60]  J. Evenden The pharmacology of impulsive behaviour in rats VII: the effects of serotonergic agonists and antagonists on responding under a discrimination task using unreliable visual stimuli , 1999, Psychopharmacology.

[61]  M. Ho,et al.  Effects of desipramine and fluvoxamine on timing behaviour investigated with the fixed-interval peak procedure and the interval bisection task , 1996, Psychopharmacology.

[62]  永福 智志 The Organization of Learning , 2005, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[63]  S. Roberts,et al.  Isolation of an internal clock. , 1981, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[64]  W. Meck,et al.  Differential modulation of clock speed by the administration of intermittent versus continuous cocaine. , 2004, Behavioral neuroscience.

[65]  C. Gallistel,et al.  Time, rate, and conditioning. , 2000, Psychological review.

[66]  J. Gibbon,et al.  Simultaneous timing of multiple intervals: implications of the scalar property. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[67]  Kimberly S. Kirkpatrick,et al.  Stimulus and temporal cues in classical conditioning. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.