Difference in sensorimotor adaptation to horizontal and vertical mirror distortions during ballistic arm movements.

When learning a novel motor task, the sensorimotor system must develop new strategies to efficiently control the limb(s) involved, and this adaptation appears to be developed through the construction of a behavioral map known as an 'internal model'. A common method to uncover the mechanisms of adaptation and reorganization processes is to expose the system to new environmental conditions, typically by introducing visual or mechanical distortions. The present study investigated the adaptation mechanisms of the human sensorimotor system to horizontal and vertical mirror distortions (HMD and VMD) during the execution of fast goal-directed arm movements. Mirror distortions (MDs) were created by means of virtual visual feedback on a computer screen while the movement was executed on a graphics tablet. Twenty healthy adult participants were recruited and assigned to one of two groups of 10 people each. Tests were divided in two subsequent blocks of five trials. The first block consisted of trials with no mirror distortion (NMD), while the second block was recorded when exposing one group to HMD and the other to VMD. Both MDs resulted in kinematic changes: during the tests with the MDs the participants did not reach the performance level found at the NMD test. Motor performance during HMD appeared to be globally better than during VMD and the adaptation process to VMD appeared to be slower than to HMD, but data interpretation was hampered by large within-participant and between-participant variability. In-depth analyses of the data revealed that most of the motor performance information was contained in the direction of movement. The data supported the idea that the internal model for HMD was already partially built.

[1]  Robert L. Sainburg,et al.  Mechanisms underlying interlimb transfer of visuomotor rotations , 2003, Experimental Brain Research.

[2]  Otmar Bock,et al.  Conditions for interference versus facilitation during sequential sensorimotor adaptation , 2001, Experimental Brain Research.

[3]  J Randall Flanagan,et al.  Visuomotor rotations of varying size and direction compete for a single internal model in motor working memory. , 2002, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[4]  R. Sainburg,et al.  Interlimb transfer of visuomotor rotations: independence of direction and final position information , 2002, Experimental Brain Research.

[5]  Philip N. Sabes,et al.  The planning and control of reaching movements , 2000, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[6]  Mitsuo Kawato,et al.  Internal models for motor control and trajectory planning , 1999, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[7]  R A Scheidt,et al.  Persistence of motor adaptation during constrained, multi-joint, arm movements. , 2000, Journal of neurophysiology.

[8]  Alexander M. Harner,et al.  Evidence for effector independent and dependent representations and their differential time course of acquisition during motor sequence learning , 2000, Experimental Brain Research.

[9]  D. Wolpert,et al.  Internal models in the cerebellum , 1998, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[10]  Reza Shadmehr,et al.  Learned dynamics of reaching movements generalize from dominant to nondominant arm. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.

[11]  John W. Krakauer,et al.  Independent learning of internal models for kinematic and dynamic control of reaching , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.

[12]  J. Flanagan,et al.  Task-specific internal models for kinematic transformations. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.

[13]  D. Chaffin,et al.  The effects of speed variation on joint kinematics during multisegment reaching movements , 1999 .

[14]  G. Stratton Vision without inversion of the retinal image. , 1897 .

[15]  Otmar Bock,et al.  Problems of sensorimotor coordination in weightlessness , 1998, Brain Research Reviews.

[16]  W. T. Thach,et al.  Prism adaptation of reaching is dependent on the type of visual feedback of hand and target position , 2001, Brain Research.

[17]  Y Burnod,et al.  Learning a new visuomotor transformation: error correction and generalization. , 1995, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[18]  R. Miall,et al.  Adaptation to rotated visual feedback: a re-examination of motor interference , 2003, Experimental Brain Research.

[19]  I Kozlovskaya,et al.  The effect of head-to-trunk position on the direction of arm movements before, during, and after space flight. , 1998, Journal of vestibular research : equilibrium & orientation.

[20]  Richard A. Heath,et al.  Space-time invariance in adult handwriting , 1997 .

[21]  Otmar Bock,et al.  Sensorimotor adaptation in young and elderly humans , 2002, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[22]  O. Bock,et al.  Sensorimotor adaptation to rotated visual input: different mechanisms for small versus large rotations , 2001, Experimental Brain Research.

[23]  J. J. Bloomberg,et al.  Context-dependent arm pointing adaptation , 2001, Behavioural Brain Research.

[24]  Otmar Bock,et al.  Transfer of sensorimotor adaptation between different movement categories , 2002, Experimental Brain Research.