Triphasic temporal impulse responses and Mach bands in time

The effect of inducing stimuli on the luminance of a brief test stimulus (2.5'-wide line) required to match the brightness of a comparison stimulus was measured at various stimulus onset asynchronies (SOAs). Assuming that the test brightness is determined by the peak response to the test stimulus, the difference in the test brightness in the absence and in the presence of an inducing stimulus as a function of the SOA was employed as a measure of the temporal response to the inducing stimulus. The temporal responses to brief decremental inducing stimuli (a 2.5'-wide line and a 30'-wide bar) consisted of three oscillations, the middle one being the largest. The triphasic form of the temporal impulse responses was confirmed by measuring the temporal step responses to these stimuli. The step responses consisted of alternating positive and negative phases which might be regarded as a temporal analogue of the Mach bands in space. The data obtained were described by a model of the weighting function which was assumed to be a spatiotemporal Gabor-like function.

[1]  J. Goodman Introduction to Fourier optics , 1969 .

[2]  R. Sekuler,et al.  Individual differences in pulse brightness perception , 1981, Perception & psychophysics.

[3]  Frans J. J. Blommaert,et al.  The foveal point spread function as a determinant for detail vision , 1981, Vision Research.

[4]  Robert Sekuler,et al.  Handbook of Sensory Physiology, Vol. 7/4, Visual Psychophysics , 1973 .

[5]  P. H. Schiller Central connections of the retinal ON and OFF pathways , 1982, Nature.

[6]  Paul G. Roofe,et al.  The Vertebrate Visual System , 1958, Neurology.

[7]  Eric G. Heinemann,et al.  Simultaneous Brightness Induction , 1972 .

[8]  A. Watson Derivation of the impulse response: comments on the method of Roufs and Blommaert , 1982, Vision Research.

[9]  D. Tolhurst,et al.  Psychophysical evidence for sustained and transient detectors in human vision , 1973, The Journal of physiology.

[10]  C. Enroth-Cugell,et al.  The contrast sensitivity of retinal ganglion cells of the cat , 1966, The Journal of physiology.

[11]  D. H. Kelly Visual responses to time-dependent stimuli. III. Individual variations. , 1962, Journal of the Optical Society of America.

[12]  U. T. Keesey Flicker and pattern detection: a comparison of thresholds. , 1972, Journal of the Optical Society of America.

[13]  D. H. Kelly Visual response to time-dependent stimuli. I. Amplitude sensitivity measurements. , 1961, Journal of the Optical Society of America.

[14]  W. Levick,et al.  Maintained Discharge in the Visual System and its Role for Information Processing , 1973 .

[15]  F. J. J. Blommaert,et al.  Temporal impulse and step responses of the human eye obtained psychophysically by means of a drift-correcting perturbation technique , 1981, Vision Research.

[16]  D. Stork,et al.  Temporal impulse responses from flicker sensitivities. , 1987, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[17]  de H Huib Ridder Dynamic properties of human brightness perception , 1987 .

[18]  W. Andrew The vertebrate visual system , 1957 .

[19]  R. W. Rodieck,et al.  Analysis of receptive fields of cat retinal ganglion cells. , 1965, Journal of neurophysiology.

[20]  C W Tyler,et al.  Psychophysical derivation of the impulse response through generation of ultrabrief responses: complex inverse estimation without minimum-phase assumptions. , 1992, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[21]  J. Movshon,et al.  Spatial summation in the receptive fields of simple cells in the cat's striate cortex. , 1978, The Journal of physiology.

[22]  David C. Burr,et al.  Impulse-response functions for chromatic and achromatic stimuli , 1993 .

[23]  J. Victor Temporal impulse responses from flicker sensitivities: causality, linearity, and amplitude data do not determine phase. , 1989, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[24]  D. Whitteridge,et al.  The representation of the visual field on the cerebral cortex in monkeys , 1961, The Journal of physiology.

[25]  C. Rashbass The visibility of transient changes of luminance , 1970, The Journal of physiology.

[26]  M. Licker Early inhibition and excitation of the retinal ganglion cells. , 1969, Vision research.

[27]  Mark A. Georgeson,et al.  Temporal properties of spatial contrast vision , 1987, Vision Research.

[28]  J. Dowling,et al.  Organization of the retina of the mudpuppy, Necturus maculosus. II. Intracellular recording. , 1969, Journal of neurophysiology.

[29]  A. Vassilev,et al.  Transient and sustained masking , 1981, Perception & psychophysics.

[30]  Mitsuo Ikeda,et al.  Temporal impulse response , 1986, Vision Research.

[31]  R. Haber,et al.  Visual Perception , 2018, Encyclopedia of Database Systems.

[32]  H. Barlow,et al.  Change of organization in the receptive fields of the cat's retina during dark adaptation , 1957, The Journal of physiology.

[33]  L. Kaufman,et al.  Handbook of perception and human performance , 1986 .

[34]  Mitsuo Ikeda,et al.  Temporal Summation of Positive and Negative Flashes in the Visual System , 1965 .

[35]  Temporal brightness enhancement studied with a large sample of observers: Evidence for individual differences in brightness perception , 1980, Perception & psychophysics.

[36]  V. Manahilov,et al.  Spatiotemporal visual response to suprathreshold stimuli , 1995, Vision Research.

[37]  ROBERT SHAPLEY,et al.  Visual spatial summation in two classes of geniculate cells , 1975, Nature.

[38]  A. Watson,et al.  Patterns of temporal interaction in the detection of gratings , 1977, Vision Research.