Internalized Gender Stereotypes Vary Across Socioeconomic Indicators

In the following we aim to approach the question of why, in most domains of professional and economic life, women are more vulnerable than men to becoming targets of prejudice and discrimination by proposing that one important cause of this inequality is the presence of gender stereotypes in many domains of society. We describe two approaches employed to measure gender stereotypes: An explicit questionnaire based on rating scales and a newly developed Implicit Association Test assessing gender stereotypes representing instrumentality (i.e., agency) and expressivity (i.e., communion). We first present information on psychometric properties of each stereotype measure designed for this purpose. We then present preliminary data based on the SOEP Innovation Sample 2011 indicating differences in explicit stereotypes with reference to occupational position and income. Implicit stereotypic associations concerning expressivity increased with respondents' age, stereotypic associations concerning instrumentality increased with household income, particularly among male participants. Finally, stereotypic associations were related simultaneously to occupational position and participants' gender, such that differences between male and female participants were found in lower occupational positions for the Expressivity IAT and in higher occupational positions for the Instrumentality IAT. This finding indicates that individually held gender stereotypes are related to socioeconomic and social variables.

[1]  Glenda Lawrence,et al.  Strategy for equality between women and men 2010-2015 , 2014 .

[2]  Jürgen Schupp,et al.  SOEP Innovation Sample (SOEP-IS) — Description, Structure and Documentation , 2012 .

[3]  A. Eagly,et al.  The Impact of Social Roles on Trait Judgments , 2012, Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin.

[4]  Rocio Garcia-Retamero,et al.  The Malleability of Gender Stereotypes: Influence of Population Size on Perceptions of Men and Women in the Past, Present, and Future , 2011, The Journal of social psychology.

[5]  A. Eagly,et al.  Are leader stereotypes masculine? A meta-analysis of three research paradigms. , 2011, Psychological bulletin.

[6]  Bertram Gawronski,et al.  Are We Puppets on a String? Comparing the Impact of Contingency and Validity on Implicit and Explicit Evaluations , 2011, Personality & social psychology bulletin.

[7]  Manfred J. Schmitt,et al.  Convergence of Direct, Indirect, and Objective Risk-Taking Measures in Gambling , 2010 .

[8]  Brian A. Nosek,et al.  National differences in gender–science stereotypes predict national sex differences in science and math achievement , 2009, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[9]  M. Banaji,et al.  Understanding and using the Implicit Association Test: III. Meta-analysis of predictive validity. , 2009, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[10]  B. Nosek Implicit–Explicit Relations , 2007 .

[11]  Brian A. Nosek,et al.  A multitrait-multimethod validation of the Implicit Association Test: implicit and explicit attitudes are related but distinct constructs. , 2007, Experimental psychology.

[12]  G. Bodenhausen,et al.  Associative and propositional processes in evaluation: an integrative review of implicit and explicit attitude change. , 2006, Psychological bulletin.

[13]  Amanda B. Diekman,et al.  Cross-Cultural Similarities and Differences in Dynamic Stereotypes: A Comparison Between Germany and the United States , 2005 .

[14]  A. Eagly,et al.  Dynamic Stereotypes about Women and Men in Latin America and the United States , 2005 .

[15]  F. Strack,et al.  Reflective and Impulsive Determinants of Social Behavior , 2004, Personality and social psychology review : an official journal of the Society for Personality and Social Psychology, Inc.

[16]  A. Eagly,et al.  Stereotypes as Dynamic Constructs: Women and Men of the Past, Present, and Future , 2000 .

[17]  Robert C. Satterwhite,et al.  Pancultural Gender Stereotypes Revisited: The Five Factor Model , 1999 .

[18]  A. Eagly,et al.  Gender-Stereotypic Images of Occupations Correspond to the Sex Segregation of Employment , 1999 .

[19]  A. Greenwald,et al.  Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: the implicit association test. , 1998, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[20]  C. Hoffman,et al.  Gender stereotypes : perception or rationalization? , 1990 .

[21]  A. Eagly,et al.  The origins of sex differences in human behavior: Evolved dispositions versus social roles , 1987 .

[22]  A. Eagly,et al.  Gender stereotypes stem from the distribution of women and men into social roles , 1984 .

[23]  R. Ashmore,et al.  Sex stereotypes and implicit personality theory: Toward a cognitive—Social psychological conceptualization , 1979 .

[24]  A. Eagly,et al.  Social role theory. , 2012 .

[25]  A. Eagly Sex differences in social behavior : a social-role interpretation , 1987 .

[26]  D. Hamilton,et al.  Stereotypes and stereotyping: An overview of the cognitive approach. , 1986 .

[27]  D. Best,et al.  Measuring Sex Stereotypes: A Multination Study , 1982 .

[28]  D. Hamilton Cognitive Processes in Stereotyping and Intergroup Behavior , 1981 .

[29]  Laurie A. Rudman,et al.  Implicit Self-Concept and Evaluative Implicit Gender Stereotypes: Self and Ingroup Share Desirable Traits , 2001 .

[30]  Wendy Wood,et al.  Provided for Non-commercial Research and Educational Use Only. Not for Reproduction, Distribution or Commercial Use. Biosocial Construction of Sex Differences and Similarities in Behavior , 2022 .