Genetic disruptions of Drosophila Pavlovian learning leave extinction learning intact

Individuals who experience traumatic events may develop persistent posttraumatic stress disorder. Patients with this disorder are commonly treated with exposure therapy, which has had limited long‐term success. In experimental neurobiology, fear extinction is a model for exposure therapy. In this behavioral paradigm, animals are repeatedly exposed in a safe environment to the fearful stimulus, which leads to greatly reduced fear. Studying animal models of extinction already has lead to better therapeutic strategies and development of new candidate drugs. Lack of a powerful genetic model of extinction, however, has limited progress in identifying underlying molecular and genetic factors. In this study, we established a robust behavioral paradigm to study the short‐term effect (acquisition) of extinction in Drosophila melanogaster. We focused on the extinction of olfactory aversive 1‐day memory with a task that has been the main workhorse for genetics of memory in flies. Using this paradigm, we show that extinction can inhibit each of two genetically distinct forms of consolidated memory. We then used a series of single‐gene mutants with known impact on associative learning to examine the effects on extinction. We find that extinction is intact in each of these mutants, suggesting that extinction learning relies on different molecular mechanisms than does Pavlovian learning.

[1]  G. McNally,et al.  The midbrain periaqueductal gray and fear extinction: opioid receptor subtype and roles of cyclic AMP, protein kinase A, and mitogen-activated protein kinase. , 2005, Behavioral neuroscience.

[2]  C. Cain,et al.  L-Type Voltage-Gated Calcium Channels Are Required for Extinction, But Not for Acquisition or Expression, of Conditional Fear in Mice , 2002, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[3]  Randolf Menzel,et al.  Spontaneous Recovery from Extinction Depends on the Reconsolidation of the Acquisition Memory in an Appetitive Learning Paradigm in the Honeybee (Apis mellifera) , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[4]  G. Quirk,et al.  Neural Mechanisms of Extinction Learning and Retrieval , 2008, Neuropsychopharmacology.

[5]  Joseph E LeDoux,et al.  Acquisition of Fear Extinction Requires Activation of NR2B-Containing NMDA Receptors in the Lateral Amygdala , 2007, Neuropsychopharmacology.

[6]  Stephen Maren,et al.  Early extinction after fear conditioning yields a context-independent and short-term suppression of conditional freezing in rats. , 2009, Learning & memory.

[7]  G. McNally,et al.  Opioid receptors regulate the extinction of Pavlovian fear conditioning. , 2003, Behavioral neuroscience.

[8]  Alcino J. Silva,et al.  Memory Reconsolidation and Extinction Have Distinct Temporal and Biochemical Signatures , 2004, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[9]  C. Rankin Context conditioning in habituation in the nematode Caenorhabditis elegans. , 2000, Behavioral neuroscience.

[10]  R. Rescorla,et al.  Spontaneous recovery varies inversely with the training-extinction interval , 2004, Learning & behavior.

[11]  Ingie Hong,et al.  Blockade of amygdala metabotropic glutamate receptor subtype 1 impairs fear extinction. , 2007, Biochemical and biophysical research communications.

[12]  N. Mons,et al.  The role of Ca2+/calmodulin-stimulable adenylyl cyclases as molecular coincidence detectors in memory formation , 1999, Cellular and Molecular Life Sciences CMLS.

[13]  A Switch from Cycloheximide-Resistant Consolidated Memory to Cycloheximide-Sensitive Reconsolidation and Extinction in Drosophila , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[14]  T. Tully,et al.  Differential effects of dunce mutations on associative learning and memory in Drosophila. , 1993, Journal of neurogenetics.

[15]  Susan Sangha,et al.  Context Extinction and Associative Learning in Lymnaea , 2002, Neurobiology of Learning and Memory.

[16]  Joëlle Lopez,et al.  Differential sensitivity of recent vs. remote memory traces to extinction in a water-maze task in rats , 2008, Neurobiology of Learning and Memory.

[17]  J. Dubnau,et al.  Deconstructing Memory in Drosophila , 2005, Current Biology.

[18]  Ann-Shyn Chiang,et al.  NMDA Receptors Mediate Olfactory Learning and Memory in Drosophila , 2005, Current Biology.

[19]  M. Bouton Context, ambiguity, and unlearning: sources of relapse after behavioral extinction , 2002, Biological Psychiatry.

[20]  M. E. Pedreira,et al.  Protein Synthesis Subserves Reconsolidation or Extinction Depending on Reminder Duration , 2003, Neuron.

[21]  J. Dubnau,et al.  Gene discovery in Drosophila: new insights for learning and memory. , 1998, Annual review of neuroscience.

[22]  M. Fanselow,et al.  Effects of naltrexone on learning and performance of conditional fear-induced freezing and opioid analgesia , 1987, Physiology & Behavior.

[23]  G. Schafe Rethinking the role of L-type voltage-gated calcium channels in fear memory extinction. , 2008, Learning & memory.

[24]  T. Préat,et al.  Genetic dissection of consolidated memory in Drosophila , 1994, Cell.

[25]  T. Tully,et al.  CREB as a Memory Modulator: induced expression of a dCREB2 activator isoform enhances long-term memory in drosophila , 1995, Cell.

[26]  W. Quinn,et al.  The Drosophila radish gene encodes a protein required for anesthesia-resistant memory , 2006, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[27]  E. Kandel,et al.  Classical conditioning in a simple withdrawal reflex in Aplysia californica , 1981, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[28]  F. James Rohlf,et al.  Biometry: The Principles and Practice of Statistics in Biological Research , 1969 .

[29]  Martin Heisenberg,et al.  Extinction Antagonizes Olfactory Memory at the Subcellular Level , 2002, Neuron.

[30]  Stephen Maren,et al.  Recent fear is resistant to extinction , 2006, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[31]  Michael Davis,et al.  Different mechanisms of fear extinction dependent on length of time since fear acquisition. , 2006, Learning & memory.

[32]  W. Quinn,et al.  Classical conditioning and retention in normal and mutantDrosophila melanogaster , 1985, Journal of Comparative Physiology A.

[33]  M. Bouton,et al.  Immediate extinction causes a less durable loss of performance than delayed extinction following either fear or appetitive conditioning. , 2008, Learning & memory.

[34]  Ronald L. Davis,et al.  The Drosophila learning and memory gene rutabaga encodes a Ca 2+ calmodulin -responsive , 1992, Cell.

[35]  Michael Davis,et al.  Facilitation of Conditioned Fear Extinction by Systemic Administration or Intra-Amygdala Infusions of d-Cycloserine as Assessed with Fear-Potentiated Startle in Rats , 2002, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[36]  Barbara O Rothbaum,et al.  Cognitive enhancers as adjuncts to psychotherapy: use of D-cycloserine in phobic individuals to facilitate extinction of fear. , 2004, Archives of general psychiatry.

[37]  Yi Ling Yang,et al.  Facilitation of conditioned fear extinction by d-cycloserine is mediated by mitogen-activated protein kinase and phosphatidylinositol 3-kinase cascades and requires de novo protein synthesis in basolateral nucleus of amygdala , 2005, Neuroscience.

[38]  Wanhe Li,et al.  Short- and Long-Term Memory in Drosophila Require cAMP Signaling in Distinct Neuron Types , 2009, Current Biology.

[39]  M. Davis,et al.  Mechanisms of fear extinction , 2007, Molecular Psychiatry.

[40]  H. Okano,et al.  rugose (rg), a Drosophila A kinase anchor protein, is required for retinal pattern formation and interacts genetically with multiple signaling pathways. , 2002, Genetics.

[41]  W. Quinn,et al.  Radish, a Drosophila mutant deficient in consolidated memory. , 1993, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[42]  S. Hofmann,et al.  Augmentation treatment of psychotherapy for anxiety disorders with D-cycloserine. , 2006, CNS drug reviews.

[43]  Michael Davis,et al.  Enhancing Cannabinoid Neurotransmission Augments the Extinction of Conditioned Fear , 2005, Neuropsychopharmacology.

[44]  M. Heisenberg Mushroom body memoir: from maps to models , 2003, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[45]  N. Singewald,et al.  Role of L-type Ca2+ channel isoforms in the extinction of conditioned fear. , 2008, Learning & memory.

[46]  W. Zieglgänsberger,et al.  The endogenous cannabinoid system controls extinction of aversive memories , 2002, Nature.

[47]  W. Quinn,et al.  Induction of a dominant negative CREB transgene specifically blocks long-term memory in Drosophila , 1994, Cell.

[48]  D. Storm,et al.  Why calcium-stimulated adenylyl cyclases? , 2004, Physiology.

[49]  W. Quinn,et al.  A neuropeptide gene defined by the Drosophila memory mutant amnesiac. , 1995, Science.

[50]  R. Muller,et al.  Consolidation of Extinction Learning Involves Transfer from NMDA-Independent to NMDA-Dependent Memory , 2001, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[51]  Thomas Preat,et al.  Exclusive Consolidated Memory Phases in Drosophila , 2004, Science.

[52]  Beat Lutz,et al.  Cannabinoid CB1 Receptor Mediates Fear Extinction via Habituation-Like Processes , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[53]  T. Abrams,et al.  Analysis of sequence-dependent interactions between transient calcium and transmitter stimuli in activating adenylyl cyclase in Aplysia: possible contribution to CS--US sequence requirement during conditioning. , 1998, Learning & memory.

[54]  S. Waddell,et al.  Drosophila olfactory memory: single genes to complex neural circuits , 2007, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[55]  Joseph E LeDoux,et al.  Evidence for recovery of fear following immediate extinction in rats and humans. , 2008, Learning & memory.

[56]  L-type voltage-gated calcium channels in conditioned fear: a genetic and pharmacological analysis. , 2008, Learning & memory.

[57]  G. McNally,et al.  Opioid Receptors in the Midbrain Periaqueductal Gray Regulate Extinction of Pavlovian Fear Conditioning , 2004, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[58]  C. Cain,et al.  Understanding Trauma: Mechanisms of Fear Extinction: Toward Improved Treatment for Anxiety , 2007 .

[59]  T. Tully,et al.  Defective Learning in Mutants of the Drosophila Gene for a Regulatory Subunit of cAMP-Dependent Protein Kinase , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[60]  Ann-Shyn Chiang,et al.  The staufen/pumilio Pathway Is Involved in Drosophila Long-Term Memory , 2003, Current Biology.

[61]  M. Livingstone,et al.  Loss of calcium/calmodulin responsiveness in adenylate cyclase of rutabaga, a Drosophila learning mutant , 1984, Cell.

[62]  Ronald L. Davis,et al.  Thirty years of olfactory learning and memory research in Drosophila melanogaster , 2005, Progress in Neurobiology.

[63]  Daniel L. Alkon,et al.  Extinction of associative learning in Hermissenda: Behavior and neural correlates , 1984, Behavioural Brain Research.

[64]  Ann-Shyn Chiang,et al.  Specific requirement of NMDA receptors for long-term memory consolidation in Drosophila ellipsoid body , 2007, Nature Neuroscience.

[65]  M. E. Pedreira,et al.  Mismatch between what is expected and what actually occurs triggers memory reconsolidation or extinction. , 2004, Learning & memory.

[66]  I. Pavlov,et al.  Conditioned reflexes: An investigation of the physiological activity of the cerebral cortex , 2010, Annals of Neurosciences.

[67]  Michael Davis,et al.  Mitogen-Activated Protein Kinase Cascade in the Basolateral Nucleus of Amygdala Is Involved in Extinction of Fear-Potentiated Startle , 2001, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[68]  James L McGaugh,et al.  The role of NMDA glutamate receptors, PKA, MAPK, and CAMKII in the hippocampus in extinction of conditioned fear , 2003, Hippocampus.

[69]  T. Tully,et al.  The AKAP Yu is required for olfactory long-term memory formation in Drosophila , 2007, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.