Interhemispheric integration of visual concepts in infancy Kimberly M. Scott (kimscott@mit.edu) Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences, MIT Cambridge, MA 02139 USA Laura E. Schulz (lschulz@mit.edu) Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences, MIT Cambridge, MA 02139 USA Abstract Abstraction often requires appropriate integration of more con- crete representations. During development, the more specific or localized representations may arise first. Here we study the special case of integration of visual representations from the left and right hemispheres during infancy. We present failures of interhemispheric integration in two domains, form percep- tion and approximate number, in infants ranging from 8 to 18 months of age. In Experiment 1, infants succeeded in repre- senting equality of two shapes only when both shapes were presented in the same visual hemifield. In Experiment 2, in- fants represented 16 when shown 16 dots in one hemifield but not when shown 8 dots in each hemifield. We argue that inter- hemispheric integration poses a particular and unusually late- resolved challenge in infant vision. Keywords: visual invariance; interhemispheric integration; corpus callosum; split-brain; approximate number system Introduction Any latent cause in the world can lead to myriad patterns of observations. Recognizing an object in the visual world requires achieving invariance over transformations–including translations, rotations, scaling, and lighting changes–that can drastically affect its retinal projection. Similarly, understand- ing more abstract concepts often requires recognizing the identity or shared causes of superficially disparate manifes- tations: three voices, three bears, or three hugs; perceptual causality or statistical covariance; a frown and a raised voice. Assuming that in some cases these specific “views” may be represented before the more general concepts, how do infants integrate these distinct representations? Newborns have already been demonstrated to have sev- eral forms of visual invariance such as size constancy (Slater, Mattock, & Brown, 1990) and recognition of invariant posi- tional relations (Antell & Caron, 1985; Milewski, 1979), al- though these progress with visual experience during the first year (McKenzie, Tootell, & Day, 1980; Granrud, 2006; Gliga & Dehaene-Lambertz, 2007). Visual adaptation and after- effects suggest that high-level visual concepts including ap- proximate number (Ross & Burr, 2010) and facial identity (Webster & Maclin, 1999) may pose similar learning chal- lenges, as they are first represented by spatially localized de- tectors. Split-brain patients likewise provide dramatic evi- dence for distinct spatially localized representations of high- level visual concepts (for review see Gazzaniga, 2005). Regardless of the exact computations involved, the rela- tionships among localized detectors need to be learned or refined to produce integrated representations. We propose to study the special case of integration of visual concepts from the left and right hemispheres. Late myelination of the corpus callosum, which connects the two cereberal hemi- spheres (Yakovlev & Lecours, 1967), its continued develop- ment through adolescence (Giedd et al., 1999; Salamy, 1978), and the separate critical period for the corpus callosum to af- fect visual development (Elberger, 1984) make this plausi- bly challenging in early childhood. Indeed, interhemispheric transfer of a visual rule learned in a single hemisphere does not occur spontaneously before 4 months of age (de Scho- nen & Bry, 1987), and children under 24 months have diffi- culty integrating information about shape across hemispheres (Liegeois, Bentejac, & de Schonen, 2000). However, this ef- fect is specific to face stimuli; the operant conditioning task introduced additional demands; and the bilateral and unilat- eral presentations differed in visual angle from the fovea, al- lowing several alternative explanations of the apparent fail- ure. We first sought to confirm infants’ difficulty comparing shapes from opposite visual hemifields. In Experiment 1, we attempted to familiarize infants with matching shapes either unilaterally or bilaterally by briefly presenting matching pairs of shapes while infants fixated on a small video. This famil- iarization period was designed to affect their preference for looking at matching shapes, which we measured before and after familiarization. Experiment 1: Is a square on the left the same shape as a square on the right? Methods Participants Infant subjects were recruited at the Boston Children’s Museum and parents provided informed consent to participate. 48 infants between 8 and 14 months of age (mean age 11 months 2 days) participated in this study. An additional 33 infants were excluded due to fussiness, inatten- tion, or experimenter error. Procedure Each infant sat on a parent’s lap for the dura- tion of the study, 1.5 m from a large monitor used to dis- play all stimuli. Subjects were videorecorded using a cam- era positioned directly above the monitor. The experimenters were positioned behind the monitor, hidden from the view of the infant, and monitored the infant using a webcam posi- tioned above the monitor while controlling the progression of the study using Psychtoolbox extensions (Brainard, 1997) in
[1]
Mark M Schira,et al.
Interactions between task difficulty and hemispheric distribution of attended locations: implications for the splitting attention debate.
,
2005,
Brain research. Cognitive brain research.
[2]
S. Schonen,et al.
Interhemispheric communication of visual learning: A developmental study in 3–6-month old infants
,
1987,
Neuropsychologia.
[3]
A. J. Caron,et al.
Neonatal perception of spatial relationships
,
1985
.
[4]
A. Gottfried,et al.
FAMILIARITY AND NOVELTY PREFERENCES IN INFANT RECOGNITION MEMORY: IMPLICATIONS FOR INFORMATION PROCESSING
,
1982
.
[5]
K. Wynn,et al.
Large-Number Addition and Subtraction by 9-Month-Old Infants
,
2004,
Psychological science.
[6]
A. Elberger,et al.
The existence of a separate, brief critical period for the corpus callosum to affect visual development
,
1984,
Behavioural Brain Research.
[7]
Stephen M. Kosslyn,et al.
Discrimination within and between hemifields: A new constraint on theories of attention
,
1991,
Neuropsychologia.
[8]
Elizabeth S. Spelke,et al.
Chronometric studies of numerical cognition in five-month-old infants
,
2005,
Cognition.
[9]
J. Tenenbaum,et al.
Infants consider both the sample and the sampling process in inductive generalization
,
2010,
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[10]
Satsuki Nakai,et al.
Distinguishing novelty and familiarity effects in infant preference procedures
,
2004
.
[11]
B. Roder,et al.
Infants' Preferences for Familiarity and Novelty During the Course of Visual Processing.
,
2000,
Infancy : the official journal of the International Society on Infant Studies.
[12]
A. Salamy,et al.
Commissural transmission: maturational changes in humans.
,
1978,
Science.
[13]
David C. Burr,et al.
Vision senses number directly
,
2009
.
[14]
Alan C. Evans,et al.
Brain development during childhood and adolescence: a longitudinal MRI study
,
1999,
Nature Neuroscience.
[15]
E. Spelke,et al.
Large number discrimination in 6-month-old infants
,
2000,
Cognition.
[16]
Fei Xu,et al.
Intuitive statistics by 8-month-old infants
,
2008,
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[17]
Michael A. Hunter,et al.
Effects of stimulus complexity and familiarization time on infant preferences for novel and familiar stimuli.
,
1983
.
[18]
P. Cavanagh,et al.
Independent Resources for Attentional Tracking in the Left and Right Visual Hemifields
,
2005,
Psychological science.
[19]
Otto H. MacLin,et al.
Figural aftereffects in the perception of faces
,
1999,
Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[20]
Frédérique Liégeois,et al.
When does inter-hemispheric integration of visual events emerge in infancy? A developmental study on 19- to 28-month-old infants
,
2000,
Neuropsychologia.
[21]
T. Gliga,et al.
Development of a view-invariant representation of the human head
,
2007,
Cognition.
[22]
A. E. Milewski,et al.
Visual Discrimination and Detection of Configurational Invariance in 3-Month Infants.
,
1979
.
[23]
M. Gazzaniga.
Forty-five years of split-brain research and still going strong
,
2005,
Nature Reviews Neuroscience.
[24]
J. Richards,et al.
Effects of attention on infants' preference for briefly exposed visual stimuli in the paired-comparison recognition-memory paradigm.
,
1997,
Developmental psychology.
[25]
C. Granrud,et al.
Size constancy in infants: 4-month-olds' responses to physical versus retinal image size.
,
2006,
Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[26]
R. Day,et al.
Development of visual size constancy during the 1st year of human infancy.
,
1980
.
[27]
P. Yakovlev,et al.
The myelogenetic cycles of regional maturation of the brain
,
1967
.
[28]
A. Slater,et al.
Size constancy at birth: newborn infants' responses to retinal and real size.
,
1990,
Journal of experimental child psychology.
[29]
Matthias M. Müller,et al.
Sustained division of spatial attention to multiple locations within one hemifield
,
2007,
Neuroscience Letters.
[30]
D. Burr,et al.
Vision senses number directly.
,
2009,
Journal of vision.
[31]
Matteo Valsecchi,et al.
Perceived numerosity is reduced in peripheral vision.
,
2013,
Journal of vision.
[32]
D H Brainard,et al.
The Psychophysics Toolbox.
,
1997,
Spatial vision.
[33]
Richard N. Aslin,et al.
The Goldilocks Effect: Human Infants Allocate Attention to Visual Sequences That Are Neither Too Simple Nor Too Complex
,
2012,
PloS one.
[34]
Karrie Karahalios,et al.
VCode and VData: illustrating a new framework for supporting the video annotation workflow
,
2008,
AVI '08.