Time and number discrimination in a bisection task with a sequence of stimuli: a developmental approach.

Children, aged 5 and 8 years, and adults were tested in a bisection task with a sequence of stimuli in which time and number co-varied. In a counting and a non-counting condition, they were instructed either to process the duration of this sequence while ignoring the number of stimuli (temporal bisection), or to process the number of stimuli while ignoring the duration (numerical bisection). In the temporal bisection task, number interfered with the 5-year-olds' temporal performance, indicating that young children did not process time and number independently in a sequence of stimuli when they had to attend to duration. However, number interference decreased both with age and counting strategy. In contrast, in the numerical bisection task, duration did not interfere with numerical discrimination for any age group.

[1]  J. Wearden,et al.  Temporal generalization in 3- to 8-year-old children. , 2001, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[2]  J. Gibbon,et al.  Human bisection at the geometric mean , 1991 .

[3]  Warren H. Meck,et al.  Temporal integration in duration and number discrimination. , 1985 .

[4]  Scott W. Brown Attentional resources in timing: Interference effects in concurrent temporal and nontemporal working memory tasks , 1997, Perception & psychophysics.

[5]  Sylvie Droit-Volet,et al.  Attentional distraction and time perception in children , 2002 .

[6]  Kelly S. Mix,et al.  Preschoolers' recognition of numerical equivalence: sequential sets. , 1999, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[7]  J. Gibbon Scalar expectancy theory and Weber's law in animal timing. , 1977 .

[8]  Avishai Henik,et al.  Automatic and intentional processing of numerical information , 1992 .

[9]  Sylvie Droit-Volet,et al.  Speeding up an Internal Clock in Children? Effects of Visual Flicker on Subjective Duration , 2002, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. B, Comparative and physiological psychology.

[10]  Simon Grondin,et al.  When to start explicit counting in a time-intervals discrimination task: A critical point in the timing process of humans. , 1999 .

[11]  E. Spelke,et al.  Large number discrimination in 6-month-old infants , 2000, Cognition.

[12]  J. Wearden,et al.  Stimulus Range Effects in Temporal Bisection by Humans , 1996, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. B, Comparative and physiological psychology.

[13]  W. Roberts Chapter 5 Does a common mechanism account for timing and counting phenomena in the pigeon , 1997 .

[14]  D. Lane,et al.  Auditory attention switching: a developmental study. , 1991, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[15]  A. Luria The Working Brain: An Introduction To Neuropsychology , 1976 .

[16]  Ewart A. C. Thomas,et al.  Cognitive processing and time perception , 1975 .

[17]  C. Gallistel,et al.  Nonverbal Counting in Humans: The Psychophysics of Number Representation , 1999 .

[18]  P. Zelazo,et al.  An age-related dissociation between knowing rules and using them ☆ , 1996 .

[19]  G. Logan Toward an instance theory of automatization. , 1988 .

[20]  H. Lejeune Switching or gating? The attentional challenge in cognitive models of psychological time , 1998, Behavioural Processes.

[21]  A. Luria,et al.  Russian Challenge. (Book Reviews: The Role of Speech in the Regulation of Normal and Abnormal Behavior) , 2014 .

[22]  J. Pascual-Leone,et al.  Developmental Measurement of Mental Attention , 1994 .

[23]  Dan Zakay,et al.  Chapter 10 Subjective Time and Attentional Resource Allocation: An Integrated Model of Time Estimation , 1989 .

[24]  J H Wearden,et al.  Temporal bisection in children. , 2001, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[25]  Sylvie Droit-Volet,et al.  Attention and time estimation in 5- and 8-year-old children: a dual-task procedure , 2002, Behavioural Processes.

[26]  F. N. Dempster,et al.  Interference and inhibition in cognition: An historical perspective , 1995 .

[27]  C. Gallistel,et al.  Non-verbal numerical cognition: from reals to integers , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[28]  R. Baayen,et al.  Affixal Homonymy triggers full-form storage, even with inflected words, even in a morphologically rich language , 2000, Cognition.

[29]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Attention, automaticity, and levels of representation in number processing , 1995 .

[30]  Matthew Flatt,et al.  PsyScope: An interactive graphic system for designing and controlling experiments in the psychology laboratory using Macintosh computers , 1993 .

[31]  S. Droit-Volet Scalar timing in temporal generalization in children with short and long stimulus durations , 2002, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[32]  A. Baddeley Human Memory: Theory and Practice, Revised Edition , 1990 .

[33]  Sylvie Droit-Volet,et al.  Temporal Experience and Timing in Children , 2003 .

[34]  David F. Bjorklund,et al.  The resources construct in cognitive development: Diverse sources of evidence and a theory of inefficient inhibition. , 1990 .

[35]  W. Meck Chapter 4 Application of a mode-control model of temporal integration to counting and timing behavior , 1997 .

[36]  R. Church,et al.  A mode control model of counting and timing processes. , 1983, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[37]  R. Church,et al.  Bisection of temporal intervals. , 1977, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[38]  W. Meck Functional and neural mechanisms of interval timing , 2003 .

[39]  Françoise Macar,et al.  Multiple approaches to investigate the existence of an internal clock using attentional resources , 1999, Behavioural Processes.

[40]  William A. Roberts,et al.  Can a pigeon simultaneously process temporal and numerical information , 1994 .

[41]  C. M. Bradshaw,et al.  Time and behaviour : psychological and neurobehavioural analyses , 1997 .

[42]  D. Bjorklund,et al.  The evolution of inhibition mechanisms and their role in human cognition and behavior , 1995 .