Pattern separation is the key driver of expectation-modulated memory

The hippocampus uses pattern separation and pattern completion in a continuous manner to successfully encode and retrieve memories1,2. However, whether and how cognitive factors might modulate the dynamics between these types of computation is not well understood. Here we examine the role of expectation in shifting the hippocampus to perform pattern separation. Expectation can be built up through multiple contextual exposures leading to prediction (as in a learnt contingency) or through logical deduction based on a previous mnemonic response. Participants first learned a contingency between a cue and an object’s category (man-made or natural). Then, at encoding, one third of the cues that preceded the to-be-memorised objects violated the studied rule. At test, participants performed an old/new recognition task with old items (targets) and a set of parametrically manipulated (very similar to dissimilar) new foils for each object. We explored the effects of both contextual expectation, manipulated at initial encoding, and mnemonic-attribution expectation, driven by the mnemonic decisions taken on previous retrieval trials. For example, a target would be unexpected if in a previous trial a similar foil had been erroneously accepted as old. Memory was found to be better for foils of high and mid similarity to contextually unexpected targets at encoding, compared to expected ones. Additionally, violations of mnemonic-attribution expectation also yielded improved memory performance when the level of foil similarity was high. These results suggest that violations of both contextual expectation and mnemonic-attribution expectation engage pattern separation, resulting in better discrimination performance for these items. Importantly, this mechanism is engaged when input differentiation is required in order to make a correct recognition decision.

[1]  James L. McClelland,et al.  Why there are complementary learning systems in the hippocampus and neocortex: insights from the successes and failures of connectionist models of learning and memory. , 1995, Psychological review.

[2]  Ellen M. Migo,et al.  Please Scroll down for Article the Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology the Contribution of Familiarity to Recognition Memory Is a Function of Test Format When Using Similar Foils , 2022 .

[3]  C. Stark,et al.  Pattern separation in the hippocampus , 2011, Trends in Neurosciences.

[4]  Michael C. Anderson,et al.  Remembering can cause forgetting: retrieval dynamics in long-term memory. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[5]  Raymond J. Dolan,et al.  Information theory, novelty and hippocampal responses: unpredicted or unpredictable? , 2005, Neural Networks.

[6]  D. Montaldi,et al.  Beneficial and detrimental effects of schema incongruence on memory for contextual events , 2018, Learning & memory.

[7]  Daniela Montaldi,et al.  How do memory systems detect and respond to novelty? , 2018, Neuroscience Letters.

[8]  Shauna M. Stark,et al.  Age-related memory deficits linked to circuit-specific disruptions in the hippocampus , 2011, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[9]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  A theory of cortical responses , 2005, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[10]  D. Kumaran,et al.  Match–Mismatch Processes Underlie Human Hippocampal Responses to Associative Novelty , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[11]  M. Hasselmo What is the function of hippocampal theta rhythm?—Linking behavioral data to phasic properties of field potential and unit recording data , 2005, Hippocampus.

[12]  R. O’Reilly,et al.  Modeling hippocampal and neocortical contributions to recognition memory: a complementary-learning-systems approach. , 2003, Psychological review.

[13]  D. Montaldi,et al.  Expectation affects learning and modulates memory experience at retrieval , 2018, Cognition.

[14]  D. Montaldi,et al.  Striatal and midbrain connectivity with the hippocampus selectively boosts memory for contextual novelty , 2015, Hippocampus.

[15]  B. Christensen,et al.  Virtual human analogs to rodent spatial pattern separation and completion memory tasks , 2011 .

[16]  Kathryn J. Jeffery,et al.  Place Cells, Grid Cells, Attractors, and Remapping , 2011, Neural plasticity.

[17]  Shauna M. Stark,et al.  Distinct pattern separation related transfer functions in human CA3/dentate and CA1 revealed using high-resolution fMRI and variable mnemonic similarity. , 2010, Learning & memory.

[18]  Chiara Braschi,et al.  Brain Plasticity and Disease: A Matter of Inhibition , 2011, Neural plasticity.

[19]  D. Shohamy,et al.  Dopamine and adaptive memory , 2010, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[20]  Michael E. Hasselmo,et al.  A Proposed Function for Hippocampal Theta Rhythm: Separate Phases of Encoding and Retrieval Enhance Reversal of Prior Learning , 2002, Neural Computation.

[21]  Randall C. O'Reilly,et al.  Theta Coordinated Error-Driven Learning in the Hippocampus , 2013, PLoS Comput. Biol..

[22]  Philip C. Ko,et al.  Memory‐related eye movements challenge behavioral measures of pattern completion and pattern separation , 2014, Hippocampus.

[23]  R Core Team,et al.  R: A language and environment for statistical computing. , 2014 .

[24]  Julia Spaniol,et al.  Spatial pattern completion deficits in older adults , 2013, Front. Ag. Neurosci..

[25]  J. Lisman,et al.  The Hippocampal-VTA Loop: Controlling the Entry of Information into Long-Term Memory , 2005, Neuron.

[26]  M. Moser,et al.  Pattern Separation in the Dentate Gyrus and CA3 of the Hippocampus , 2007, Science.

[27]  R. Henson,et al.  Does prediction error drive one-shot declarative learning? , 2017, Journal of memory and language.

[28]  Nicole M. Long,et al.  Hippocampal Mismatch Signals Are Modulated by the Strength of Neural Predictions and Their Similarity to Outcomes , 2016, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[29]  U. Frey,et al.  Hippocampal synaptic plasticity: role in spatial learning or the automatic recording of attended experience? , 1997, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[30]  Moshe Bar,et al.  The proactive brain: memory for predictions , 2009, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[31]  Véronique D. Bohbot,et al.  Spatial navigational strategies correlate with gray matter in the hippocampus of healthy older adults tested in a virtual maze , 2013, Front. Ag. Neurosci..

[32]  N. Daw,et al.  Integrating memories to guide decisions , 2015, Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences.

[33]  J. Raaijmakers,et al.  Rethinking inhibition theory: On the problematic status of the inhibition theory for forgetting , 2013 .

[34]  D. Kumaran,et al.  An Unexpected Sequence of Events: Mismatch Detection in the Human Hippocampus , 2006, PLoS biology.

[35]  Oliver J Gray,et al.  SOLID-Similar object and lure image database , 2019, Behavior Research Methods.

[36]  R. Rescorla,et al.  A theory of Pavlovian conditioning : Variations in the effectiveness of reinforcement and nonreinforcement , 1972 .

[37]  Jonathan W. Peirce,et al.  PsychoPy—Psychophysics software in Python , 2007, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[38]  M. Moser,et al.  Understanding memory through hippocampal remapping , 2008, Trends in Neurosciences.

[39]  C. Stark,et al.  Pattern Separation in the Human Hippocampal CA3 and Dentate Gyrus , 2008, Science.

[40]  D. Bates,et al.  Fitting Linear Mixed-Effects Models Using lme4 , 2014, 1406.5823.