Favoured genetic background for testing anxiolytics in the fear-potentiated and light-enhanced startle paradigms in the rat

The fear-potentiated startle (FPS) and the light-enhanced startle (LES) paradigms are rodent tests of fear and anxiety, which combine face validity with predictive validity for clinically effective anxiolytic drugs. However, systematic strain comparisons aimed at identifying a rat strain that shows robust and reliable fear and anxiety responses in both models are missing. Here, we investigated four commonly used laboratory rat strains: Wistar, Sprague Dawley, Long-Evans and F344. Following strong cued fear conditioning training [60 conditioned stimulus-unconditioned stimulus (CS-US) pairings], all strains except Wistar exhibited significant FPS responses. F344 rats showed the strongest FPS response. Following milder cued fear conditioning protocols, designed to reduce the underlying component of contextual fear conditioning (by context pre-exposure or less CS-US pairings), also Wistar rats were able to show significant FPS, albeit still to a lesser extent than F344 rats tested under identical conditions. When tested in the LES protocol (light intensity ∼ 1500 lx), all strains except Long-Evans displayed significant light-enhanced startle responses. F344 and Wistar showed the strongest LES responses, which were of similar magnitude. The most sensitive strain in both paradigms, F344, was chosen for further pharmacological validation. The clinically active anxiolytic alprazolam (0.3, 1, 3mg/kg p.o.) dose-dependently reduced both fear-like responses in the FPS paradigm and anxiety-like responses in the LES paradigm at non-myorelaxant dosages. We propose that the F344 rat strain is particularly suited for the predictability assessment of novel anxiolytic drugs in both startle paradigms.

[1]  Michael Davis Pharmacological analysis of fear-potentiated startle. , 1993, Brazilian journal of medical and biological research = Revista brasileira de pesquisas medicas e biologicas.

[2]  Michael Davis,et al.  Double Dissociation between the Involvement of the Bed Nucleus of the Stria Terminalis and the Central Nucleus of the Amygdala in Startle Increases Produced by Conditioned versus Unconditioned Fear , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[3]  P. Groves,et al.  Habituation: a dual-process theory. , 1970, Psychological review.

[4]  C. Grillon,et al.  Darkness facilitates the acoustic startle reflex in humans , 1997, Biological Psychiatry.

[5]  Michael Davis,et al.  Anxiogenic effects of high illumination levels assessed with the acoustic startle response in rats , 1997, Biological Psychiatry.

[6]  Michael Davis,et al.  Are different parts of the extended amygdala involved in fear versus anxiety? , 1998, Biological Psychiatry.

[7]  Michael Davis,et al.  Quantifying fear potentiated startle using absolute versus proportional increase scoring methods: implications for the neurocircuitry of fear and anxiety , 2002, Psychopharmacology.

[8]  C. Grillon Models and mechanisms of anxiety: evidence from startle studies , 2008, Psychopharmacology.

[9]  Matthias J. Wieser,et al.  Darkness-enhanced startle responses in ecologically valid environments: A virtual tunnel driving experiment , 2008, Biological Psychology.

[10]  M. Koch,et al.  The acoustic startle response in rats—circuits mediating evocation, inhibition and potentiation , 1997, Behavioural Brain Research.

[11]  Michael Davis,et al.  Neural systems involved in fear and anxiety measured with fear-potentiated startle. , 2006, The American psychologist.

[12]  Christian Grillon,et al.  A review of the modulation of the startle reflex by affective states and its application in psychiatry , 2003, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[13]  T. Hijzen,et al.  Fear-potentiated startle response is remarkably similar in two laboratories , 1996, Psychopharmacology.

[14]  P. Pilz,et al.  Comparison of Two Sensitization Paradigms of the Acoustic Startle Response in Wistar and Sprague–Dawley Rats , 1999, Behavior genetics.

[15]  B. Olivier,et al.  The light-enhanced startle paradigm as a putative animal model for anxiety: effects of chlordiazepoxide, flesinoxan and fluvoxamine , 2002, Psychopharmacology.

[16]  C. Grillon,et al.  Comments on the use of the startle reflex in psychopharmacological challenges: impact of baseline startle on measurement of fear-potentiated startle , 2002, Psychopharmacology.

[17]  Michael Davis,et al.  The dorsal cochlear nucleus contributes to a high intensity component of the acoustic startle reflex in rats , 1998, Hearing Research.

[18]  J. Glowa,et al.  Differences in response to an acoustic startle stimulus among forty-six rat strains , 1994, Behavior genetics.

[19]  G. Muir,et al.  Fischer (F-344) rats have different morphology, sensorimotor and locomotor abilities compared to Lewis, Long–Evans, Sprague–Dawley and Wistar rats , 2003, Behavioural Brain Research.

[20]  Michael Davis,et al.  Pharmacological and anatomical analysis of fear conditioning using the fear-potentiated startle paradigm. , 1986, Behavioral neuroscience.

[21]  M. Fendt,et al.  The neuroanatomical and neurochemical basis of conditioned fear , 1999, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[22]  J. S. Brown,et al.  Conditioned fear as revealed by magnitude of startle response to an auditory stimulus. , 1951, Journal of experimental psychology.

[23]  M. Davis,et al.  Differential Effects of the CRF-R1 Antagonist GSK876008 on Fear-Potentiated, Light- and CRF-Enhanced Startle Suggest Preferential Involvement in Sustained versus Phasic Threat Responses , 2008, Neuropsychopharmacology.

[24]  A. Rodríguez-Fornells,et al.  Effects of alprazolam on the acoustic startle response in humans , 1999, Psychopharmacology.

[25]  J. Gugten,et al.  Light-enhanced and fear-potentiated startle: temporal characteristics and effects of α-helical corticotropin-releasing hormone , 2003, Biological Psychiatry.

[26]  M. Geyer,et al.  Differential startle amplitude and corticosterone response in rats. , 1992, Neuroendocrinology.