Genetic and Environmental Contributions to the Development of Childhood Aggression

Longitudinal data from a large sample of twins participating in the Netherlands Twin Register (n = 42,827, age range 3–16) were analyzed to investigate the genetic and environmental contributions to childhood aggression. Genetic auto-regressive (simplex) models were used to assess whether the same genes are involved or whether new genes come into play as children grow up. The authors compared 2 different simplex models to disentangle potentially changing behavioral expressions from changes in genetic and environmental effects. One model provided estimates of genetic and environmental effects at the level of individual aggression questionnaire items, and the other model assessed the effects at the level of an aggression sum score computed from the individual items. The results from both models provided evidence for largely stable genetic effects throughout childhood. The results also highlighted the differential heritability of the different indicators of aggression measured with the Childhood Behavior Checklist, with destruction of property showing a very high genetic component during early childhood and fighting behaviors being more heritable in early adolescence.

[1]  L. R. Huesmann,et al.  Stability of Aggression Over Time and Generations. , 1984 .

[2]  Naomi R. Wray,et al.  Genetic Studies of Major Depressive Disorder: Why Are There No Genome-wide Association Study Findings and What Can We Do About It? , 2014, Biological Psychiatry.

[3]  D. Boomsma,et al.  Causes of Stability of Aggression from Early Childhood to Adolescence: A Longitudinal Genetic Analysis in Dutch Twins , 2003, Behavior genetics.

[4]  Meike Bartels,et al.  Genetic Contributions to Subtypes of Aggression , 2005, Twin Research and Human Genetics.

[5]  D. I. Boomsma,et al.  Co-occurrence of Aggressive Behavior and Rule-Breaking Behavior at Age 12: Multi-Rater Analyses , 2003, Behavior genetics.

[6]  P. Scheet,et al.  The Young Netherlands Twin Register (YNTR): Longitudinal Twin and Family Studies in Over 70,000 Children , 2012, Twin Research and Human Genetics.

[7]  Meike Bartels,et al.  Twins and the study of rater (dis)agreement. , 2007, Psychological methods.

[8]  P. Sham,et al.  Longitudinal heritability of childhood aggression , 2016, American journal of medical genetics. Part B, Neuropsychiatric genetics : the official publication of the International Society of Psychiatric Genetics.

[9]  Gitta H Lubke,et al.  Genetic Analyses Benefit From Using Less Heterogeneous Phenotypes: An Illustration With the Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale (HADS) , 2015, Genetic epidemiology.

[10]  C. Duijn,et al.  Genome-wide analyses of borderline personality features , 2013, Molecular Psychiatry.

[11]  Kevin J. Grimm,et al.  Modeling life-span growth curves of cognition using longitudinal data with multiple samples and changing scales of measurement. , 2009, Psychological methods.

[12]  B. Byrne,et al.  Testing for the equivalence of factor covariance and mean structures: The issue of partial measurement invariance. , 1989 .

[13]  M. Neale,et al.  Implications of absence of measurement invariance for detecting sex limitation and genotype by environment interaction. , 2004, Twin research : the official journal of the International Society for Twin Studies.

[14]  J. Mcardle,et al.  Alternative common factor models for multivariate biometric analyses , 1990, Behavior genetics.

[15]  Michael C. Rodriguez,et al.  Assessing Relational and Physical Aggression Among Preschool Children , 2003 .

[16]  N. Martin,et al.  Symptoms of anxiety and symptoms of depression. Same genes, different environments? , 1987, Archives of general psychiatry.

[17]  A. Raine,et al.  Aggression and Rule-breaking: Heritability and stability of antisocial behavior problems in childhood and adolescence. , 2013, Journal of criminal justice.

[18]  Justin M. Luningham,et al.  Sum Scores in Twin Growth Curve Models: Practicality Versus Bias , 2017, Behavior genetics.

[19]  Dorret I. Boomsma,et al.  A Note on the Statistical Power in Extended Twin Designs , 2000, Behavior genetics.

[20]  G. Lubke,et al.  Inference Based on the Best-Fitting Model Can Contribute to the Replication Crisis: Assessing Model Selection Uncertainty Using a Bootstrap Approach , 2016, Structural equation modeling : a multidisciplinary journal.

[21]  D. Hay,et al.  Key issues in the development of aggression and violence from childhood to early adulthood. , 1997, Annual review of psychology.

[22]  E. Viding,et al.  Developmentally dynamic genome: Evidence of genetic influences on increases and decreases in conduct problems from early childhood to adolescence , 2015, Scientific Reports.

[23]  R. Althoff,et al.  Classes of oppositional-defiant behavior: concurrent and predictive validity. , 2014, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[24]  H. Steiner,et al.  Aggression and Antisocial Behavior in Children and Adolescents: Research and Treatment , 2003 .

[25]  M. Mosher,et al.  Relational and overt aggression in preschool. , 1997, Developmental psychology.

[26]  R. Tremblay,et al.  A longitudinal twin study of physical aggression during early childhood: evidence for a developmentally dynamic genome , 2014, Psychological Medicine.

[27]  D I Boomsma,et al.  The genetic analysis of repeated measures. I. Simplex models , 1987, Behavior genetics.

[28]  D. Boomsma,et al.  Young Netherlands Twin Register (Y-NTR): A Longitudinal Multiple Informant Study of Problem Behavior , 2007, Twin Research and Human Genetics.

[29]  William Meredith,et al.  The role of factorial invariance in modeling growth and change. , 2001 .

[30]  A. Raine,et al.  Genetic and environmental stability differs in reactive and proactive aggression. , 2009, Aggressive behavior.

[31]  K. Björkqvist,et al.  Sex differences in physical, verbal, and indirect aggression: A review of recent research , 1994 .

[32]  T. Eley,et al.  Aetiological Influences on Stability and Change in Emotional and Behavioural Problems across Development: A Systematic Review , 2017, Psychopathology review.

[33]  L J Eaves,et al.  The genetical analysis of covariance structure , 1977, Heredity.

[34]  G. J. Lewis,et al.  Heritable influences on behavioural problems from early childhood to mid-adolescence: evidence for genetic stability and innovation , 2015, Psychological Medicine.