The Perception and Cognition of Environmental Distance: Direct Sources of Information

Research on direct sources of information for the perception and cognition of environmental distance is reviewed. Environmental distances are relatively large and cannot be perceived in entirety from a single place.Directly-acquired knowledge of environmental distance is based on the sensorimotor apprehension of information fromthe body or from the environment during locomotion. The paper is organized around the idea that distance knowledge is derived from multiple, partially redundant information sources. After briefly discussing general issues, I review literature on direct sources of distance information,including environmental features, travel time, and travel effort. Theories of how these sources provide information about distance are considered. To date, only information from environmental features has received much empirical support as a direct source of distance knowledge, particularly to the extent that the features segment routes into vistas. Key weaknesses in the empirical literatureare discussed, and importantresearch directions are identified.

[1]  C. D. Smith,et al.  The relationship between the pleasingness of landmarks and the judgement of distance in cognitive maps , 1984 .

[2]  D R Montello,et al.  ROUTE INFORMATION AND TRAVEL TIME AS BASES FOR THE PERCEPTION AND COGNITION OF ENVIRONMENTAL DISTANCE , 1988 .

[3]  R. Golledge,et al.  Spatial Behavior: A Geographic Perspective , 1996 .

[4]  G. Allen,et al.  Effects of the cognitive organization of route knowledge on judgments of macrospatial distance , 1985, Memory & cognition.

[5]  Barbara Hayes-Roth,et al.  Differences in spatial knowledge acquired from maps and navigation , 1982, Cognitive Psychology.

[6]  J. Rieser,et al.  The role of visual experience in knowledge of spatial layout , 1980, Perception & psychophysics.

[7]  R Job,et al.  Schematic information, attention, and memory for places. , 1983, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[8]  Jack Brimberg,et al.  A New Distance Function for Modeling Travel Distances in a Transportation Network , 1992, Transp. Sci..

[9]  G. Allen A developmental perspective on the effects of "subdividing" macrospatial experience. , 1981 .

[10]  Richard A. Block,et al.  Remembered duration: Evidence for a contextual-change hypothesis. , 1978 .

[11]  E. Sadalla,et al.  Reference points in spatial cognition. , 1980, Journal of experimental psychology. Human learning and memory.

[12]  John Cohen,et al.  New Phenomena in Apparent Duration, Distance and Speed , 1962, Nature.

[13]  Gordon O. Ewing,et al.  ON THE SENSITIVITY OF CONCLUSIONS ABOUT THE BASES OF COGNITIVE DISTANCE , 1981 .

[14]  G. Evans,et al.  Environment and Behavior , 2001 .

[15]  Robert Dale,et al.  Proceedings of the Thirteenth Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society , 1991 .

[16]  Pat Burnett,et al.  Time Cognition and Urban Travel Behavior , 1978 .

[17]  D. R. Montello A New Framework for Understanding the Acquisition of Spatial Knowledge in Large-Scale Environments , 1998 .

[18]  W. H. Ittelson Environment and cognition , 1974 .

[19]  Alan M. MacEachren,et al.  TRAVEL TIME AS THE BASIS OF COGNITIVE DISTANCE , 1980 .

[20]  Max J. Egenhofer,et al.  Spatial and temporal reasoning in geographic information systems , 1998 .

[21]  D. R. Montello,et al.  Remembering Changes in Direction , 1989 .

[22]  Harry Heft The Vicissitudes of Ecological Phenomena in Environment-Behavior Research , 1988 .

[23]  R. Downs,et al.  Maps in minds : reflections on cognitive mapping , 1978 .

[24]  R. Klatzky,et al.  Acquisition of route and survey knowledge in the absence of vision. , 1990, Journal of motor behavior.

[25]  N. Newcombe,et al.  Barrier effects in the cognitive maps of children and adults. , 1982, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[26]  W. Jeffrey Burroughs,et al.  Retrieval processes in distance cognition , 1979 .

[27]  Daniel R. Montello,et al.  The measurement of cognitive distance: Methods and construct validity , 1991 .

[28]  José Aparecido da Silva Scales for subjective distance in a large open field from the fractionation method: effects of type of judgment and distance range. , 1982 .

[29]  Jack L. Nasar,et al.  Out of Sight Further from Mind , 1985 .

[30]  John C. Baird,et al.  Psychophysical analysis of visual space , 1974 .

[31]  O. Bratfisch,et al.  Subjective distance and emotional involvement. A psychological mechanism. , 1965, Acta psychologica.

[32]  Wesley A. Mah,et al.  Cognitive reference points in judgments of symbolic magnitude , 1982, Cognitive Psychology.

[33]  S J Lederman,et al.  Exploring environments by hand or foot: time-based heuristics for encoding distance in movement space. , 1987, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[34]  John T. Coshall Urban Consumers' Cognitions of Distance , 1985 .

[35]  Y Tsal,et al.  The role of segmentation in prospective and retrospective time estimation processes , 1994, Memory & cognition.

[36]  Robert Cohen,et al.  Cognitive Maps of a Naturalistic Setting. , 1978 .

[37]  N. Gale,et al.  Exploring the anchor-point hypothesis of spatial cognition , 1987 .

[38]  Louis Buffardi,et al.  Factors affecting the filled-duration illusion in the auditory, tactual, and visual modalities , 1971 .

[39]  E. Sadalla,et al.  The Perception of Traversed Distance , 1980 .

[40]  Richard A. Block,et al.  Cognitive models of psychological time , 1992 .

[41]  George Lakoff,et al.  Women, Fire, and Dangerous Things , 1987 .

[42]  S. Kosslyn,et al.  Cognitive maps in children and men. , 1974, Child development.

[43]  J. Jonides,et al.  Evidence of hierarchies in cognitive maps , 1985, Memory & cognition.

[44]  D. Canter,et al.  Distance Estimation in Cities , 1975 .

[45]  Christine A. Klein,et al.  Children's Distance Estimates in a Large-Scale Environment , 1986 .