An evaluation of statistical procedures for comparing an individual's performance with that of a group of controls

The single case methodology that is widely used in cognitive neuropsychology often requires a comparison of data from a single individual (the patient) with that from a group of controls, in order to ascertain whether the patient's mean score can be viewed as significantly different from that of controls. This article reviews methods that have been used to deal with such data. Although Analysis of Variance (ANOVA) provides one possible solution of comparing group means, unequal group sizes and differences in variability between patient and controls can violate the assumptions of the test. Using Monte Carlo simulations, it was found that differences in group size and a group of N = 1 did not significantly affect the reliability of the analysis. In contrast, unacceptably high Type I errors were obtained when, in addition to unequal group sizes, there were relatively modest differences between the variance of the patient and that of the controls. We suggest that ANOVA can be used for the comparison of the mean score of an individual with that of a group of controls, but that when there is a difference in variability between the two groups, revised F criteria should be used in order to make the analysis reliable. A table of modified F values is given, which can be used for various departures from homogeneity of variance.

[1]  Anastasia L. Raymer,et al.  Dyssynchronous apraxia: failure to combine simultaneous preprogrammed movements. , 1998, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[2]  M. Behrmann,et al.  Visual complexity in letter-by-letter reading: Pure alexia is not pure , 1998, Neuropsychologia.

[3]  D. C. Howell Statistical methods for psychology, 3rd ed. , 1992 .

[4]  D. Bergel Geigy Scientific Tables , 1991 .

[5]  H. H. Clark The language-as-fixed-effect fallacy: A critique of language statistics in psychological research. , 1973 .

[6]  William H. Press,et al.  Numerical recipes in C , 2002 .

[7]  S. S. Wilks Determination of Sample Sizes for Setting Tolerance Limits , 1941 .

[8]  Lorraine K. Tyler,et al.  When leopards lose their spots: knowledge of visual properties in category-specific deficits for living things , 1997 .

[9]  Martin Arguin,et al.  EXTENT AND LIMITS OF COVERT LEXICAL ACTIVATION IN LETTER-BY-LETTER READING. , 1998, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[10]  T. Shallice,et al.  Confabulation with a selective descriptor process impairment , 1999 .

[11]  D. C. Howell Statistical Methods for Psychology , 1987 .

[12]  Donald E. Knuth,et al.  The art of computer programming: V.1.: Fundamental algorithms , 1997 .

[13]  AN ACQUIRED FORM OF DEVELOPMENTAL PHONOLOGICAL DYSLEXIA , 1999 .

[14]  C. A. Boneau,et al.  The effects of violations of assumptions underlying the test. , 1960, Psychological bulletin.

[15]  Donald E. Knuth,et al.  The Art of Computer Programming, Volume I: Fundamental Algorithms, 2nd Edition , 1997 .

[16]  Abraham Wald,et al.  An Extension of Wilks' Method for Setting Tolerance Limits , 1943 .

[17]  A. Caramazza,et al.  The case for single-patient studies , 1988 .

[18]  D. C. Howell,et al.  Comparing an Individual's Test Score Against Norms Derived from Small Samples , 1998 .

[19]  Giuseppe Zappalà,et al.  The mini‐mental state examination: Normative study of an Italian random sample , 1993 .

[20]  Tim Shallice,et al.  Case study approach in neuropsychological research , 1979 .

[21]  Bruno Laeng,et al.  CAN DEFICITS IN SPATIAL INDEXING CONTRIBUTE TO SIMULTANAGNOSIA , 1999 .

[22]  Glyn W. Humphreys,et al.  The magic number four and temporo-parietal damage: Neurological impairments in counting targets amongst distractors. , 1999 .

[23]  Jamie Ward,et al.  False recognition after frontal lobe damage: The role of encoding factors , 1999 .

[24]  E. Capitani,et al.  How can we evaluate interference in attentional tests? A study based on bi-variate non-parametric tolerance limits. , 1999, Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology.