A randomized comparison of patients’ understanding of number needed to treat and other common risk reduction formats

BACKGROUND: Commentators have suggested that patients may understand quantitative information about treatment benefits better when they are presented as numbers needed to treat (NNT) rather than as absolute or relative risk reductions. OBJECTIVE: To determine whether NNT helps patients interpret treatment benefits better than absolute risk reduction (ARR), relative risk reduction (RRR), or a combination of all three of these risk reduction presentations (COMBO). DESIGN: Randomized cross-sectional survey. SETTING: University internal medicine clinic. PATIENTS: Three hundred fifty-seven men and women, ages 50 to 80, who presented for health care. INTERVENTIONS: Subjects were given written information about the baseline risk of a hypothetical “disease Y” and were asked (1) to compare the benefits of two drug treatments for disease Y, stating which provided more benefit; and (2) to calculate the effect of one of those drug treatments on a given baseline risk of disease. Risk information was presented to each subject in one of four randomly allocated risk formats: NNT, ARR, RRR, or COMBO. MAIN RESULTS: When asked to state which of two treatments provided more benefit, subjects who received the RRR format responded correctly most often (60% correct vs 43% for COMBO, 42% for ARR, and 30% for NNT, P=.001). Most subjects were unable to calculate the effect of drug treatment on the given baseline risk of disease, although subjects receiving the RRR and ARR formats responded correctly more often (21% and 17% compared to 7% for COMBO and 6% for NNT, P=.004). CONCLUSION: Patients are best able to interpret the benefits of treatment when they are presented in an RRR format with a given baseline risk of disease. ARR also is easily interpreted. NNT is often misinterpreted by patients and should not be used alone to communicate risk to patients.

[1]  C D Naylor,et al.  Communicating the Benefits of Chronic Preventive Therapy , 1995, Medical decision making : an international journal of the Society for Medical Decision Making.

[2]  Lisa M. Schwartz,et al.  The Role of Numeracy in Understanding the Benefit of Screening Mammography , 1997, Annals of Internal Medicine.

[3]  G H Guyatt,et al.  Users' guides to the medical literature: XX. Integrating research evidence with the care of the individual patient. Evidence-Based Medicine Working Group. , 2000, JAMA.

[4]  T. Walley,et al.  Same information, different decisions: the influence of evidence on the management of hypertension in the elderly. , 1996, The British journal of general practice : the journal of the Royal College of General Practitioners.

[5]  Nancy L. Kocovski,et al.  Perception of Quantitative Information for Treatment Decisions , 2000, Medical decision making : an international journal of the Society for Medical Decision Making.

[6]  B. Rimer,et al.  General Performance on a Numeracy Scale among Highly Educated Samples , 2001, Medical decision making : an international journal of the Society for Medical Decision Making.

[7]  D L Sackett,et al.  An assessment of clinically useful measures of the consequences of treatment. , 1988, The New England journal of medicine.

[8]  J. Robertson,et al.  Effects of information framing on the intentions of family physicians to prescribe long-term hormone replacement therapy , 1999, Journal of General Internal Medicine.

[9]  R M Arnold,et al.  Absolutely relative: how research results are summarized can affect treatment decisions. , 1992, The American journal of medicine.

[10]  A. B. Gustafson,et al.  Number needed to treat is a simple measure of treatment efficacy for clinicians , 1996, Journal of General Internal Medicine.

[11]  Lisa M. Schwartz,et al.  How can we help people make sense of medical data? , 1999, Effective clinical practice : ECP.

[12]  B. Demichelis,et al.  Completeness of reporting trial results: effect on physicians' willingness to prescribe , 1994, The Lancet.

[13]  K. Gyr,et al.  Influence of method of reporting study results on decision of physicians to prescribe drugs to lower cholesterol concentration , 1994, BMJ.

[14]  D. Sackett Inference and decision at the bedside. , 1989, Journal of Clinical Epidemiology.

[15]  D. Grimes,et al.  Patients' understanding of medical risks: implications for genetic counseling. , 1999, Obstetrics and gynecology.

[16]  C D Naylor,et al.  Measured Enthusiasm: Does the Method of Reporting Trial Results Alter Perceptions of Therapeutic Effectiveness? , 1992, Annals of Internal Medicine.

[17]  J. Hutton,et al.  Number needed to treat: properties and problems , 2000 .

[18]  B Fischhoff,et al.  A New Scale for Assessing Perceptions of Chance , 2000, Medical decision making : an international journal of the Society for Medical Decision Making.

[19]  Peter Salovey,et al.  The Influence of Message Framing on Intentions to Perform Health Behaviors , 1993 .