10 Action-Oriented Models of Cognitive Processing A Little Less Cogitation , A Little More Action Please

This chapter considers action-oriented processing from a model-oriented standpoint. Possible relationships between action and cognition are reviewed in abstract or conceptual terms. We then turn to models of their interrelationships and role in mediating cognitively enriched behaviors. Examples of theories or models inspired by the actionoriented paradigm are briefl y surveyed, with a particular focus on ideomotor theory and how it has developed over the past century. Formal versions of these theories are introduced, drawing on formulations in systems biology, information theory, and dynamical systems theory. An attempt is made to integrate these perspectives under the enactivist version of the Bayesian brain; namely, active inference. Implications of this formalism and, more generally, of action-oriented views of cognition are discussed, and open issues that may be usefully pursued from a formal perspective are highlighted. Existing Schema for Action and Cognition Before considering the form and consequences of models that take an actionoriented view of cognition, it is worth considering how the relationship between these two processes has been described. Here, we consider four schemata which capture different notions of how cognition and action could be coupled (Figure 10.1). It is important to stress that these schemata are not models but From “The Pragmatic Turn: Toward Action-Oriented Views in Cognitive Science,” Andreas K. Engel, Karl J. Friston, and Danica Kragic, eds. 2016. Strüngmann Forum Reports, vol. 18, series ed. J. Lupp. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. ISBN 978-0-262-03432-6. 160 J. Kilner et al. rather depictions of different views on cognition and action. Crucially, differences in the schemata do not refl ect a fundamental difference in the nature of the coupling but rather in how cognition and action are defi ned. At one extreme, in Figure 10.1a, the two processes can be considered to be entirely independent processes: action is simply the behavioral output of the cognitive process (e.g., in the classical sandwich conception of the mind). In this open-loop formulation, action does not, and indeed cannot, infl uence cognition. Although not explicitly stated, the scheme depicted in Figure 10.1a is implicitly assumed in many models of high-level human cognitive functions used in behavioral and imaging experiments: input is carefully manipulated and output (action) is kept to a minimum (e.g., key pressing) in order to focus on internal processes. Here, action is considered to be a necessary output to disclose internal operations. For example, in a typical experiment that investigates language processing, individuals are presented with written or spoken words/sentences and asked to make a key press decision about them. Such studies, which still constitute the majority of cognitive science and neuroscience studies of high-level cognition, are completely silent as to whether action might play a role. Alternative approaches (Figure 10.1b–d) consider how action and cognition depend on each other. In addition, action can be explicitly considered to be a subset of cognition or a largely overlapping process (Figure 10.1b and c). This commonly held view defi nes cognition as information processing à la Neisser. Accordingly, most, but not all, cognition relates to action: cognition infl uences action, and action infl uences cognition. This infl uence is typically, but not always, constructive: cognition and action can exist in harmony without collapsing into one another. They are heavily intertwined, but they are not the same, and none exists solely for the benefi t of the other. Just like attention, working memory, and consciousness, cognition and action are intimately related, yet (a)

[1]  G. Vigliocco,et al.  Language as a multimodal phenomenon: implications for language learning, processing and evolution , 2014, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[2]  Friedemann Pulvermüller,et al.  From sensorimotor learning to memory cells in prefrontal and temporal association cortex: A neurocomputational study of disembodiment , 2014, Cortex.

[3]  Aslı Özyürek,et al.  Hearing and seeing meaning in speech and gesture: insights from brain and behaviour , 2014, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[4]  Linda B. Smith,et al.  Joint Attention without Gaze Following: Human Infants and Their Parents Coordinate Visual Attention to Objects through Eye-Hand Coordination , 2013, PloS one.

[5]  Martin J. Pickering,et al.  How tightly are production and comprehension interwoven? , 2013, Front. Psychol..

[6]  B. Hommel,et al.  Intentional weighting: a basic principle in cognitive control , 2012, Psychological research.

[7]  Ray Jackendoff,et al.  What is the human language faculty?: Two views , 2011 .

[8]  John K. Tsotsos A Computational Perspective on Visual Attention , 2011 .

[9]  John K. Tsotsos,et al.  Visual search for an object in a 3D environment using a mobile robot , 2010, Comput. Vis. Image Underst..

[10]  L. Fadiga,et al.  Active perception: sensorimotor circuits as a cortical basis for language , 2010, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[11]  Susan Goldin-Meadow,et al.  When gesture-speech combinations do and do not index linguistic change , 2009, Language and cognitive processes.

[12]  Csr Young,et al.  How to Do Things With Words , 2009 .

[13]  Jürgen Jost,et al.  External and internal complexity of complex adaptive systems , 2004, Theory in Biosciences.

[14]  Hermann Lotze Medicinische Psychologie, oder, Physiologie der Seele , 2006 .

[15]  Geoffrey E. Hinton,et al.  Inferring Motor Programs from Images of Handwritten Digits , 2005, NIPS.

[16]  John K. Tsotsos On the relative complexity of active vs. passive visual search , 2004, International Journal of Computer Vision.

[17]  G. Aschersleben,et al.  The Theory of Event Coding (TEC): a framework for perception and action planning. , 2001, The Behavioral and brain sciences.

[18]  Michael A. Arbib,et al.  Synthetic brain imaging: grasping, mirror neurons and imitation , 2000, Neural Networks.

[19]  M. Arbib,et al.  Grasping objects: the cortical mechanisms of visuomotor transformation , 1995, Trends in Neurosciences.

[20]  John K. Tsotsos Behaviorist Intelligence and the Scaling Problem , 1995, Artif. Intell..

[21]  John K. Tsotsos,et al.  Active object recognition , 1992, Proceedings 1992 IEEE Computer Society Conference on Computer Vision and Pattern Recognition.

[22]  Jürgen Schmidhuber,et al.  Curious model-building control systems , 1991, [Proceedings] 1991 IEEE International Joint Conference on Neural Networks.

[23]  J. Fuster,et al.  Prefrontal Cortex and the Bridging of Temporal Gaps in the Perception‐Action Cycle , 1990, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[24]  A. Koller,et al.  Speech Acts: An Essay in the Philosophy of Language , 1969 .

[25]  R. Held,et al.  MOVEMENT-PRODUCED STIMULATION IN THE DEVELOPMENT OF VISUALLY GUIDED BEHAVIOR. , 1963, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.