Prolonged Aftereffect of Visuomotor Adaptation to Gradually Distorted Reality Displayed on a See-Through Head-Mounted Device

ABSTRACT Growing evidence suggests that the gradual transformation of visuomotor association drives a distinct learning process from abrupt transformation in humans. In the current study, we developed a novel omnidirectional visuomotor transformation paradigm to study details of such difference in more realistic environment than conventional experimental systems. Participants were asked to perform a repetitive three-dimensional (3D) arm-reaching task to a target on a front touch panel, wearing a video see-through head-mounted device that displayed a rotating view of surrounding images. In the abrupt condition, the images were rotated by 20°; in the gradual condition, the rotation was increased in a stepwise-manner from 0° to 20°. In both conditions, pointing errors were decreased after adaptation. Further, although the aftereffect of adaptation was not different between conditions, the speed of decay of the aftereffect, which was quantified by an exponential fit, was slower in the gradual condition, suggesting longer-lasting aftereffects for the gradual shift.

[1]  Sarah E. Criscimagna-Hemminger,et al.  Size of error affects cerebellar contributions to motor learning. , 2010, Journal of neurophysiology.

[2]  S. Lehman The Neural and Behavioural Organization of Goal‐Directed Movements , 1990, Neurology.

[3]  Konrad Paul Kording,et al.  Estimating the sources of motor errors for adaptation and generalization , 2008, Nature Neuroscience.

[4]  G. Stelmach,et al.  Adaptation to gradual as compared with sudden visuo-motor distortions , 1997, Experimental Brain Research.

[5]  Jordan A Taylor,et al.  Explicit and Implicit Processes Constitute the Fast and Slow Processes of Sensorimotor Learning , 2015, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[6]  Konrad P. Körding,et al.  Uncertainty of Feedback and State Estimation Determines the Speed of Motor Adaptation , 2009, Front. Comput. Neurosci..

[7]  Yves Rossetti,et al.  Long-lasting aftereffect of a single prism adaptation: shifts in vision and proprioception are independent , 2006, Experimental Brain Research.

[8]  Jörn Diedrichsen,et al.  Reach adaptation: what determines whether we learn an internal model of the tool or adapt the model of our arm? , 2008, Journal of neurophysiology.

[9]  R. C. Oldfield The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. , 1971, Neuropsychologia.

[10]  Jun Rekimoto,et al.  JackIn Head: Immersive Visual Telepresence System with Omnidirectional Wearable Camera , 2015, IEEE Transactions on Visualization and Computer Graphics.

[11]  Yves Rossetti,et al.  Enhancing Visuomotor Adaptation by Reducing Error Signals: Single-step (Aware) versus Multiple-step (Unaware) Exposure to Wedge Prisms , 2007, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[12]  R. Shadmehr,et al.  Modulation of error-sensitivity during a prism adaptation task in people with cerebellar degeneration. , 2015, Journal of neurophysiology.

[13]  Maurice A. Smith,et al.  Environmental Consistency Determines the Rate of Motor Adaptation , 2014, Current Biology.

[14]  G. Torres-Oviedo,et al.  Natural error patterns enable transfer of motor learning to novel contexts. , 2012, Journal of neurophysiology.

[15]  Mark Shelhamer,et al.  Sensorimotor adaptation error signals are derived from realistic predictions of movement outcomes. , 2011, Journal of neurophysiology.

[16]  Markus Neuhäuser,et al.  Permutation Tests , 2011, International Encyclopedia of Statistical Science.

[17]  J. Krakauer,et al.  An Implicit Plan Overrides an Explicit Strategy during Visuomotor Adaptation , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[18]  Per Capita,et al.  About the authors , 1995, Machine Vision and Applications.

[19]  J. Flanagan,et al.  Learning and recall of incremental kinematic and dynamic sensorimotor transformations , 2005, Experimental Brain Research.

[20]  D. Ostry,et al.  Sensory Plasticity in Human Motor Learning , 2016, Trends in Neurosciences.

[21]  Hiroaki Gomi,et al.  Multiple Motor Learning Strategies in Visuomotor Rotation , 2010, PloS one.

[22]  J. Krakauer,et al.  Adaptation to Visuomotor Transformations: Consolidation, Interference, and Forgetting , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[23]  Daichi Nozaki,et al.  Cross talk in implicit assignment of error information during bimanual visuomotor learning. , 2011, Journal of neurophysiology.

[24]  R. C. Oldfield THE ASSESSMENT AND ANALYSIS OF HANDEDNESS , 1971 .

[25]  R. Shadmehr,et al.  Interacting Adaptive Processes with Different Timescales Underlie Short-Term Motor Learning , 2006, PLoS biology.

[26]  M. Jeannerod The neural and behavioural organization of goal-directed movements , 1990, Psychological Medicine.

[27]  Andrew A G Mattar,et al.  Motor learning and its sensory effects: time course of perceptual change and its presence with gradual introduction of load. , 2013, Journal of neurophysiology.

[28]  Junya Masumoto,et al.  Visual and Proprioceptive Adaptation of Arm Position in a Virtual Environment , 2015, Journal of motor behavior.

[29]  Vincent S. Huang,et al.  Persistence of motor memories reflects statistics of the learning event. , 2009, Journal of neurophysiology.

[30]  Reza Shadmehr,et al.  A memory of errors in sensorimotor learning , 2014, Science.

[31]  Sarah E. Criscimagna-Hemminger,et al.  Contributions of the motor cortex to adaptive control of reaching depend on the perturbation schedule. , 2011, Cerebral cortex.

[32]  John E. Schlerf,et al.  Dynamic Modulation of Cerebellar Excitability for Abrupt, But Not Gradual, Visuomotor Adaptation , 2012, The Journal of Neuroscience.