Second-order motion shifts perceived position

Many studies have documented that first-order motion influences perceived position. Here, we show that second-order (contrast defined) motion influences the perceived positions of stationary objects as well. We used a Gabor pattern as our second-order stimulus, which consisted of a drifting sinusoidal contrast modulation of a dynamic random-dot background; this second-order carrier was enveloped by a static Gaussian contrast modulation. Two vertically aligned Gabors had carrier motion in opposite directions. Subjects judged the relative positions of the Gabors' static envelopes. The positions of the Gabors appeared shifted in the direction of the carrier motion, but the effect was narrowly tuned to low temporal frequencies across all tested spatial frequencies. In contrast, first-order (luminance defined) motion shifted perceived positions across a wide range of temporal frequencies, and this differential tuning could not be explained by differences in the visibility of the patterns. The results show that second-order motion detection mechanisms contribute to perceived position. Further, the differential spatial and temporal tuning of the illusion supports the idea that there are distinct position assignment mechanisms for first and second-order motion.

[1]  P. Cavanagh,et al.  Position-based motion perception for color and texture stimuli: effects of contrast and speed , 1999, Vision Research.

[2]  M J Morgan,et al.  Spatiotemporal Filtering and the Interpolation Effect in Apparent Motion , 1980, Perception.

[3]  S. Nishida,et al.  Motion aftereffect with flickering test patterns reveals higher stages of motion processing , 1995, Vision Research.

[4]  Johannes M. Zanker,et al.  Perceptual deformation induced by visual motion , 2001, Naturwissenschaften.

[5]  Y Dan,et al.  Motion-Induced Perceptual Extrapolation of Blurred Visual Targets , 2001, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[6]  R. Snowden,et al.  Shifts in perceived position following adaptation to visual motion , 1998, Current Biology.

[7]  Nicolaas Prins,et al.  On the perceived location of global motion , 2002, Vision Research.

[8]  P. Cavanagh,et al.  Motion: the long and short of it. , 1989, Spatial vision.

[9]  K. D. De Valois,et al.  Vernier acuity with stationary moving Gabors. , 1991, Vision research.

[10]  A J Ahumada,et al.  Model of human visual-motion sensing. , 1985, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[11]  Patrick Cavanagh,et al.  The motion-induced position shift depends on the perceived direction of bistable quartet motion , 2004, Vision Research.

[12]  David Whitaker,et al.  Motion Adaptation Distorts Perceived Visual Position , 2002, Current Biology.

[13]  Katsumi Watanabe,et al.  The motion-induced position shift depends on the visual awareness of motion , 2005, Vision Research.

[14]  John Ross,et al.  Direct Evidence That “Speedlines” Influence Motion Mechanisms , 2002, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[15]  J. Lewis,et al.  Probit Analysis (3rd ed). , 1972 .

[16]  David C. Burr,et al.  Smooth and sampled motion , 1986, Vision Research.

[17]  E H Adelson,et al.  Spatiotemporal energy models for the perception of motion. , 1985, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[18]  Patrick Cavanagh,et al.  Early binding of feature pairs for visual perception , 2001, Nature Neuroscience.

[19]  D. H. Kelly Motion and vision. II. Stabilized spatio-temporal threshold surface. , 1979, Journal of the Optical Society of America.

[20]  David Whitney,et al.  Motion distorts visual space: shifting the perceived position of remote stationary objects , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[21]  Allan C. Dobbins,et al.  Quantitative depth perception of surfaces with multiple matches , 2004 .

[22]  Wilson S. Geisler,et al.  Motion streaks provide a spatial code for motion direction , 1999, Nature.

[23]  S. McKee,et al.  Statistical properties of forced-choice psychometric functions: Implications of probit analysis , 1985, Perception & psychophysics.

[24]  V. Ramachandran,et al.  Illusory Displacement of Equiluminous Kinetic Edges , 1990, Perception.

[25]  P Cavanagh,et al.  Attention-based motion perception. , 1992, Science.

[26]  M. Morgan Pulfrich Effect and the Filling in of Apparent Motion , 1976, Perception.

[27]  S. Nishida,et al.  Simultaneous motion contrast across space: Involvement of second-order motion? , 1997, Vision Research.

[28]  ANDREW T SMITH,et al.  Separate Detection of Moving Luminance and Contrast Modulations: Fact or Artifact? , 1997, Vision Research.

[29]  D. Badcock,et al.  Global motion perception: No interaction between the first- and second-order motion pathways , 1995, Vision Research.

[30]  Chuan Yi Tang,et al.  A 2.|E|-Bit Distributed Algorithm for the Directed Euler Trail Problem , 1993, Inf. Process. Lett..

[31]  David R. Badcock,et al.  Motion distorts perceived depth , 2003, Vision Research.

[32]  Jeremy M Wolfe,et al.  Attentional pursuit is faster than attentional saccade. , 2004, Journal of vision.

[33]  Andrew T. Smith,et al.  Evidence for separate motion-detecting mechanisms for first- and second-order motion in human vision , 1994, Vision Research.

[34]  S. Nishida Motion-Based Analysis of Spatial Patterns by the Human Visual System , 2004, Current Biology.

[35]  J. van Santen,et al.  Elaborated Reichardt detectors. , 1985, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[36]  Takashi R Sato,et al.  Perceived Shifts of Flashed Stimuli by Visible and Invisible Object Motion , 2003, Perception.

[37]  P. Cavanagh,et al.  Position displacement, not velocity, is the cue to motion detection of second-order stimuli , 1998, Vision Research.

[38]  David Whitaker,et al.  Non-veridical size perception of expanding and contracting objects , 1999, Vision Research.

[39]  M. Lappe,et al.  Neuronal latencies and the position of moving objects , 2001, Trends in Neurosciences.

[40]  D. Burr Acuity for apparent vernier offset , 1979, Vision Research.

[41]  Frans A. J. Verstraten,et al.  Limits of attentive tracking reveal temporal properties of attention , 2000, Vision Research.

[42]  James E. McCarthy Directional adaptation effects with contrast modulated stimuli , 1993, Vision Research.

[43]  T. Poggio,et al.  Visual hyperacuity: spatiotemporal interpolation in human vision , 1981, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B. Biological Sciences.

[44]  A. Derrington,et al.  Separate detectors for simple and complex grating patterns? , 1985, Vision Research.

[45]  Paul V McGraw,et al.  Motion-Sensitive Neurones in V5/MT Modulate Perceived Spatial Position , 2004, Current Biology.

[46]  Fang Fang,et al.  Strong influence of test patterns on the perception of motion aftereffect and position. , 2004, Journal of vision.

[47]  David Whitney,et al.  The influence of visual motion on perceived position , 2002, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[48]  D. Burr,et al.  Contrast sensitivity at high velocities , 1982, Vision Research.

[49]  Szonya Durant,et al.  Temporal dependence of local motion induced shifts in perceived position , 2004, Vision Research.

[50]  P. Cavanagh,et al.  A minimum motion technique for judging equiluminance , 1983 .

[51]  David Whitney,et al.  Motion distorts perceived position without awareness of motion , 2005, Current Biology.

[52]  David Whitney,et al.  Flexible retinotopy: motion-dependent position coding in the visual cortex. , 2010, Science.

[53]  George Sperling,et al.  Attention-generated apparent motion , 1995, Nature.

[54]  Frans A. J. Verstraten,et al.  The Motion Aftereffect:A Modern Perspective , 1998 .

[55]  Shinsuke Shimojo,et al.  Shifts in perceived position of flashed stimuli by illusory object motion , 2002, Vision Research.

[56]  Kenneth R. Boff,et al.  Vernier offset produced by rotary target motion , 1976 .

[57]  Shin'ya Nishida,et al.  Influence of motion signals on the perceived position of spatial pattern , 1999, Nature.