Attention in Active Vision: A Perspective on Perceptual Continuity Across Saccades
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] Martin Rolfs,et al. Reach preparation enhances visual performance and appearance , 2013, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[2] Jan Theeuwes,et al. A reinvestigation of the reference frame of the tilt-adaptation aftereffect , 2013, Scientific Reports.
[3] M Concetta Morrone,et al. Neural mechanisms for timing visual events are spatially selective in real-world coordinates , 2007, Nature Neuroscience.
[4] P. Cavanagh,et al. The gender-specific face aftereffect is based in retinotopic not spatiotopic coordinates across several natural image transformations. , 2009, Journal of vision.
[5] J R Duhamel,et al. The updating of the representation of visual space in parietal cortex by intended eye movements. , 1992, Science.
[6] Edward Awh,et al. A Neural Measure of Precision in Visual Working Memory , 2013, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[7] Eric Castet,et al. Motion perception of saccade-induced retinal translation , 2002, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[8] James W Bisley,et al. Anticipatory Remapping of Attentional Priority across the Entire Visual Field , 2012, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[9] Inci Ayhan,et al. Retinotopic adaptation-based visual duration compression. , 2010, Journal of vision.
[10] David E. Irwin,et al. Evidence against visual integration across saccadic eye movements , 1983, Perception & psychophysics.
[11] D. Burr,et al. Selective suppression of the magnocellular visual pathway during saccadic eye movements , 1994, Nature.
[12] Heiner Deubel,et al. Attentional selection during preparation of prehension movements , 2003 .
[13] Ronald A. Rensink,et al. TO SEE OR NOT TO SEE: The Need for Attention to Perceive Changes in Scenes , 1997 .
[14] D. E. Irwin,et al. Covert shifts of attention precede involuntary eye movements. , 2010, Perception & psychophysics.
[15] Daniel J. Simons,et al. Inattentional blindness , 2007, Scholarpedia.
[16] Lawrence H Snyder,et al. The Responses of Visual Neurons in the Frontal Eye Field Are Biased for Saccades , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[17] M. Morgan. A bias-free measure of retinotopic tilt adaptation. , 2014, Journal of vision.
[18] A. L. I︠A︡rbus. Eye Movements and Vision , 1967 .
[19] B. Dosher,et al. The role of attention in the programming of saccades , 1995, Vision Research.
[20] J. O'Regan,et al. Integrating visual information from successive fixations:Does trans-saccadic fusion exist? , 1983, Vision Research.
[21] H. Deubel,et al. Saccade target selection and object recognition: Evidence for a common attentional mechanism , 1996, Vision Research.
[22] Alexandre Zénon,et al. Attention deficits without cortical neuronal deficits , 2012, Nature.
[23] D. E. Irwin,et al. Eye movements and scene perception: Memory for things observed , 2002, Perception & psychophysics.
[24] D. Melcher. Spatiotopic Transfer of Visual-Form Adaptation across Saccadic Eye Movements , 2005, Current Biology.
[25] Ashleigh M. Richard,et al. Understanding the function of visual short-term memory: transsaccadic memory, object correspondence, and gaze correction. , 2008, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[26] E. Maris,et al. Movement preparation improves touch perception without awareness , 2015, Cognition.
[27] P. Wenderoth,et al. Retinotopic encoding of the direction aftereffect , 2008, Vision Research.
[28] Patrick Cavanagh,et al. The reference frame of the motion aftereffect is retinotopic. , 2009, Journal of vision.
[29] T. Pasternak,et al. Working memory in primate sensory systems , 2005, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.
[30] Jillian H. Fecteau,et al. Salience, relevance, and firing: a priority map for target selection , 2006, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[31] J. O'Regan,et al. Solving the "real" mysteries of visual perception: the world as an outside memory. , 1992, Canadian journal of psychology.
[32] Patrick Cavanagh,et al. Attentional trade-offs maintain the tracking of moving objects across saccades. , 2015, Journal of neurophysiology.
[33] P. Cavanagh,et al. Spatial constancy of attention across eye movements is mediated by the presence of visual objects , 2015, Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics.
[34] Bart Krekelberg,et al. Summation of Visual Motion across Eye Movements Reflects a Nonspatial Decision Mechanism , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[35] H. Deubel,et al. Independent Allocation of Attention to Eye and Hand Targets in Coordinated Eye-Hand Movements , 2011, Psychological science.
[36] James R Müller,et al. Microstimulation of the superior colliculus focuses attention without moving the eyes. , 2005, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[37] Etienne Olivier,et al. A Deficit in Covert Attention after Parietal Cortex Inactivation in the Monkey , 2004, Neuron.
[38] A. Belopolsky,et al. Target–Distractor Competition in the Oculomotor System Is Spatiotopic , 2014, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[39] J. Theeuwes,et al. Parallel allocation of attention prior to the execution of saccade sequences. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[40] David C. Burr,et al. A generalized sense of number , 2014, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[41] Christopher B. Currie,et al. Visual stability across saccades while viewing complex pictures. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[42] Edward F. Ester,et al. PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Research Article Stimulus-Specific Delay Activity in Human Primary Visual Cortex , 2022 .
[43] Eric Castet,et al. Spatiotemporal dynamics of visual attention during saccade preparation: Independence and coupling between attention and movement planning. , 2007, Journal of vision.
[44] David E. Irwin,et al. The role of the saccade target object in the perception of a visually stable world , 2000, Perception & psychophysics.
[45] B. Bridgeman,et al. Failure to integrate visual information from successive fixations , 1983 .
[46] R. Andersen,et al. Coding of intention in the posterior parietal cortex , 1997, Nature.
[47] M. Goldberg,et al. The representation of visual salience in monkey parietal cortex , 1998, Nature.
[48] Brian S. Schnitzer,et al. Eye movements and attention: The role of pre-saccadic shifts of attention in perception, memory and the control of saccades , 2012, Vision Research.
[49] David Burr,et al. Spatiotopic perceptual maps in humans: evidence from motion adaptation , 2012, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[50] Julie D. Golomb,et al. The Native Coordinate System of Spatial Attention Is Retinotopic , 2008, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[51] Patrick Cavanagh,et al. Object Recognition: Visual Crowding from a Distance , 2013, Current Biology.
[52] M. Carrasco,et al. Perceptual enhancement of contrast by attention , 2004 .
[53] W. Schneider,et al. Predicting object features across saccades: evidence from object recognition and visual search. , 2014, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[54] Richard J Krauzlis,et al. Inactivation of primate superior colliculus impairs covert selection of signals for perceptual judgments , 2010, Nature Neuroscience.
[55] Jason B. Mattingley,et al. Visual Crowding at a Distance during Predictive Remapping , 2013, Current Biology.
[56] Patrick Cavanagh,et al. The reference frame of the tilt aftereffect. , 2011, Journal of vision.
[57] P. Cz.. Handbuch der physiologischen Optik , 1896 .
[58] R. McPeek,et al. The eye dominates in guiding attention during simultaneous eye and hand movements. , 2011, Journal of vision.
[59] Etienne Olivier,et al. Contribution of the Monkey Frontal Eye Field to Covert Visual Attention , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[60] Jan Theeuwes,et al. Evidence for the predictive remapping of visual attention , 2009, Experimental Brain Research.
[61] P. Cavanagh,et al. Looking ahead: the perceived direction of gaze shifts before the eyes move. , 2009, Journal of vision.
[62] Patrick Cavanagh,et al. Phase-encoded fmri investigation of retinotopic remapping responses , 2010 .
[63] H. Deubel,et al. Delayed Saccades, but Not Delayed Manual Aiming Movements, Require Visual Attention Shifts , 2003, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.
[64] F. Hamker. The reentry hypothesis: the putative interaction of the frontal eye field, ventrolateral prefrontal cortex, and areas V4, IT for attention and eye movement. , 2005, Cerebral cortex.
[65] T. Moore,et al. Microstimulation of the frontal eye field and its effects on covert spatial attention. , 2004, Journal of neurophysiology.
[66] Denis Schluppeck,et al. Decoding Working Memory of Stimulus Contrast in Early Visual Cortex , 2013, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[67] M. Carrasco,et al. Rapid Simultaneous Enhancement of Visual Sensitivity and Perceived Contrast during Saccade Preparation , 2012, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[68] David E. Irwin. Information integration across saccadic eye movements , 1991, Cognitive Psychology.
[69] H. Deubel,et al. Selective Dorsal and Ventral Processing: Evidence for a Common Attentional Mechanism in Reaching and Perception , 1998 .
[70] Robert H. Wurtz,et al. Subcortical Modulation of Attention Counters Change Blindness , 2004, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[71] Katherine M. Armstrong,et al. Selective gating of visual signals by microstimulation of frontal cortex , 2003, Nature.
[72] William Curran,et al. Direction-contingent duration compression is primarily retinotopic , 2014, Vision Research.
[73] Brian S. Schnitzer,et al. Attentional enhancement along the path of a sequence of saccades , 2010 .
[74] Patrick Cavanagh,et al. Remapped visual masking. , 2011, Journal of vision.
[75] M. Goldberg,et al. Spatial processing in the monkey frontal eye field. I. Predictive visual responses. , 1997, Journal of neurophysiology.
[76] F. Tong,et al. Decoding reveals the contents of visual working memory in early visual areas , 2009, Nature.
[77] Ronald A. Rensink,et al. Change blindness: past, present, and future , 2005, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[78] Joo-Hyun Song,et al. Deficits in reach target selection during inactivation of the midbrain superior colliculus , 2011, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[79] Eckart Zimmermann,et al. Buildup of spatial information over time and across eye-movements , 2014, Behavioural Brain Research.
[80] Martin Rolfs,et al. Adaptive deployment of spatial and feature-based attention before saccades , 2013, Vision Research.
[81] J. Reynolds,et al. Attentional modulation of visual processing. , 2004, Annual review of neuroscience.
[82] Tirin Moore,et al. Rapid enhancement of visual cortical response discriminability by microstimulation of the frontal eye field , 2007, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[83] O'Regan Jk,et al. Integrating visual information from successive fixations: does trans-saccadic fusion exist? , 1983 .
[84] Maria Concetta Morrone,et al. Constructing Stable Spatial Maps of the Word , 2012, Perception.
[85] J. Grimes. On the failure to detect changes in scenes across saccades. , 1996 .
[86] P. Cavanagh,et al. Visual stability based on remapping of attention pointers , 2010, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[87] J. Bisley. The neural basis of visual attention , 2011, The Journal of physiology.
[88] Arash Afraz,et al. Topography of the motion aftereffect with and without eye movements. , 2008, Journal of vision.
[89] D. E. Irwin. Integrating Information Across Saccadic Eye Movements , 1996 .
[90] Patrick Cavanagh,et al. Allocation of attention across saccades. , 2013, Journal of neurophysiology.
[91] M. Carrasco. Visual attention: The past 25 years , 2011, Vision Research.
[92] M. Carrasco,et al. Attention alters appearance , 2004, Nature Neuroscience.
[93] Jacqueline Gottlieb,et al. Functional Significance of Nonspatial Information in Monkey Lateral Intraparietal Area , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[94] I. Rock,et al. Perception without attention: Results of a new method , 1992, Cognitive Psychology.
[95] P H Schiller,et al. Visual representations during saccadic eye movements. , 1998, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[96] T Moore,et al. Control of eye movements and spatial attention. , 2001, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[97] Heiner Deubel,et al. Properties of attentional selection during the preparation of sequential saccades , 2007, Experimental Brain Research.
[98] K. Rayner,et al. Is visual information integrated across saccades? , 1983, Perception & psychophysics.
[99] Y. Sugita,et al. Surround-contingent tilt aftereffect. , 2014, Journal of vision.
[100] A. L. Yarbus,et al. Eye Movements and Vision , 1967, Springer US.
[101] Samuel U Nummela,et al. Attention points to the future , 2011, Nature Neuroscience.
[102] R. Wurtz,et al. Brain circuits for the internal monitoring of movements. , 2008, Annual review of neuroscience.
[103] M. Goldberg,et al. Neurons in the monkey superior colliculus predict the visual result of impending saccadic eye movements. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.
[104] G. Sperling,et al. Information transfer in iconic memory experiments. , 1993, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[105] David Melcher,et al. Spatiotopic temporal integration of visual motion across saccadic eye movements , 2003, Nature Neuroscience.
[106] P. Cavanagh,et al. Predictive remapping of attention across eye movements , 2011, Nature Neuroscience.