Prepulse inhibition of perceived stimulus intensity: paradigm assessment

In 1939, Helen Peak reported that the presentation of paired acoustic stimuli, separated by 177ms, resulted in a 25% reduction in the perceived intensity of the second stimulus. After 65 years, this form of prepulse inhibition of perceived stimulus intensity (PPIPSI) remains relatively less developed, compared to technically and analytically complex "gating" measures, e.g. PPI of startle and P50 event related potential (ERP) suppression. We report that unlike PPI, which can be observed at very brief inter-stimulus intervals (ISI's), PPIPSI requires ISI's >or=60ms. It exhibits significant test-retest stability over a 2-week period, does not exhibit sex differences or menstrual cyclicity, and is not significantly associated with personality dimensions of novelty seeking, harm avoidance, reward dependence or sensation seeking. PPIPSI is a robust, stable, parametrically sensitive and "low tech" measure of sensory gating that is relatively unaffected by a number of biological variables in normal individuals.

[1]  M. Teghtsoonian,et al.  Consistency of individual exponents in cross-modal matching , 1983, Perception & psychophysics.

[2]  A. Mcghie,et al.  Disorders of attention and perception in early schizophrenia. , 1961, The British journal of medical psychology.

[3]  D. E. Walker,et al.  Premonitory urges in Tourette's syndrome. , 1993, The American journal of psychiatry.

[4]  N. Swerdlow,et al.  Effects of amantadine and bromocriptine on startle and sensorimotor gating: parametric studies and cross-species comparisons , 2002, Psychopharmacology.

[5]  N. Swerdlow,et al.  Central dopamine hyperactivity in rats mimics abnormal acoustic startle response in schizophrenics , 1986, Biological Psychiatry.

[6]  Kristin S Cadenhead,et al.  Prepulse inhibition and habituation of the startle response are stable neurobiological measures in a normal male population , 1999, Biological Psychiatry.

[7]  N. Swerdlow,et al.  Prestimulus modification of the startle reflex: relationship to personality and physiological markers of dopamine function , 2003, Biological Psychology.

[8]  N. Swerdlow,et al.  Men are more inhibited than women by weak prepulses , 1993, Biological Psychiatry.

[9]  J. Leckman,et al.  Sensory phenomena associated with Gilles de la Tourette's syndrome. , 1992, The Journal of clinical psychiatry.

[10]  N. Swerdlow,et al.  Assessing the validity of an animal model of deficient sensorimotor gating in schizophrenic patients. , 1994, Archives of general psychiatry.

[11]  R Freedman,et al.  Inhibitory gating of an evoked response to repeated auditory stimuli in schizophrenic and normal subjects. Human recordings, computer simulation, and an animal model. , 1996, Archives of general psychiatry.

[12]  L. Marks,et al.  Time course of loudness recalibration: implications for loudness enhancement. , 2003, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[13]  M. Teghtsoonian,et al.  Children's scales of length and loudness: a developmental application of cross-modal matching. , 1980, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[14]  J. Rotrosen,et al.  Menstrual cycle phase effects on prepulse inhibition of acoustic startle. , 2004, Psychophysiology.

[15]  M. K. Wynne,et al.  Measurements of loudness growth in 1/2-octave bands for children and adults with normal hearing. , 1999 .

[17]  R. Hellman,et al.  Prediction of individual loudness exponents from cross-modality matching. , 1988, Journal of speech and hearing research.

[18]  D. Braff,et al.  Do self-reports of perceptual anomalies reflect gating deficits in schizophrenia patients? , 2000, Biological Psychiatry.

[19]  R. Hellman Cross-modality matching: a tool for measuring loudness in sensorineural impairment. , 1999, Ear and hearing.

[20]  M. Geyer,et al.  Low dose ketamine increases prepulse inhibition in healthy men , 2003, Neuropharmacology.

[21]  S. Potkin,et al.  Is P50 suppression a measure of sensory gating in schizophrenia? , 1998, Biological Psychiatry.

[22]  S. Rauch,et al.  Sensory phenomena in obsessive-compulsive disorder and Tourette's disorder. , 2000, The Journal of clinical psychiatry.

[23]  Erica Duncan,et al.  Clinical and Sensorimotor Gating Effects of Ketamine in Normals , 2001, Neuropsychopharmacology.

[24]  H. Peak Time order error in successive judgments and in reflexes. I. Inhibition of the judgment and the reflex , 1939 .

[25]  F. Graham,et al.  Presidential Address, 1974. The more or less startling effects of weak prestimulation. , 1975, Psychophysiology.

[26]  A A Collins,et al.  The measurement of loudness in individual children and adults by absolute magnitude estimation and cross-modality matching. , 1989, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[27]  D Mangelsdorff,et al.  What is the sensation seeker? Personality trait and experience correlates of the Sensation-Seeking Scales. , 1972, Journal of consulting and clinical psychology.

[28]  Prepulse effects on magnitude estimation of startle-eliciting stimuli and startle responses , 1996, Perception & psychophysics.

[29]  Dennis S. Charney,et al.  Neuropsychopharmacology : The Fifth Generation of Progress , 2002 .

[30]  N. Schiavetti,et al.  Acoustic and psychophysical dimensions of the perceived speech naturalness of nonstutterers and posttreatment stutterers. , 1990, The Journal of speech and hearing disorders.

[31]  N. Swerdlow,et al.  Prestimulus effects on startle magnitude: sensory or motor? , 2002, Behavioral neuroscience.

[32]  Derek A. Hamilton,et al.  Blocking in human place learning: Evidence from virtual navigation , 1999, Psychobiology.

[33]  Dragan M. Svrakic,et al.  The Tridimensional Personality Questionnaire: U.S. Normative Data , 1991, Psychological reports.

[34]  T. Blumenthal,et al.  Prepulses Reduce the Pain of Cutaneous Electrical Shocks , 2001, Psychosomatic medicine.

[35]  C. R. Cloninger,et al.  A psychobiological model of temperament and character. , 1993, Archives of general psychiatry.

[36]  N. Swerdlow,et al.  The neural substrates of sensorimotor gating of the startle reflex: a review of recent findings and their implications , 1992, Journal of psychopharmacology.

[37]  N. Swerdlow,et al.  Matching strategies for drug studies of prepulse inhibition in humans , 2001, Behavioural pharmacology.

[38]  Interactions between test- and inducer-tone durations in induced loudness reduction. , 2003, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[39]  N. Swerdlow,et al.  Changes in sensorimotor inhibition across the menstrual cycle: Implications for neuropsychiatric disorders , 1997, Biological Psychiatry.

[40]  L E Marks,et al.  Cross-modality matching functions generated by magnitude estimation , 1980, Perception & psychophysics.

[41]  Howard S. Hoffman,et al.  Sensory magnitude estimation in the context of reflex modification. , 1981 .

[42]  R Freedman,et al.  Neurophysiological evidence for a defect in neuronal mechanisms involved in sensory gating in schizophrenia. , 1982, Biological psychiatry.

[43]  M. Geyer,et al.  Prestimulus effects on human startle reflex in normals and schizophrenics. , 1978, Psychophysiology.

[44]  W. Perlstein,et al.  Lead stimulation effects on reflex blink, exogenous brain potentials, and loudness judgments. , 1993, Psychophysiology.

[45]  M. Dawson,et al.  Attentional modulation of short- and long-lead-interval modification of the acoustic startle eyeblink response: comparing auditory and visual prestimuli. , 1999, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.