Working memory across nostrils.

Whether olfactory working memory involves verbal representations or neural images of odor per se remains unclear. This study investigated whether verbal representation influences performance in an olfactory delayed-match-to-sample task and used monorhinal presentation to generate hypotheses as to the underlying anatomy of this mechanism. The main findings were that (a) nameable odorants were easier to remember than hard-to-name odorants and (b) the nameability effect was more pronounced when the evaluation was done across nostrils. Considering these results within a proposed model implies dual representation in olfactory working memory: All odors, nameable and hard to name, are represented both perceptually and verbally.

[1]  O. Jensen,et al.  Modulation of Gamma and Alpha Activity during a Working Memory Task Engaging the Dorsal or Ventral Stream , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[2]  Noam Sobel,et al.  A comparison of methods for sniff measurement concurrent with olfactory tasks in humans. , 2006, Chemical senses.

[3]  Christopher Miles,et al.  Serial position effects in recognition memory for odors: A reexamination , 2005, Memory & cognition.

[4]  Jessica Porter,et al.  Brain Mechanisms for Extracting Spatial Information from Smell , 2005, Neuron.

[5]  Clayton E. Curtis,et al.  Maintenance of Spatial and Motor Codes during Oculomotor Delayed Response Tasks , 2004, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[6]  G. Paxinos,et al.  THE HUMAN NERVOUS SYSTEM , 1975 .

[7]  K. White,et al.  Demystifying wine expertise: olfactory threshold, perceptual skill and semantic memory in expert and novice wine judges. , 2002, Chemical senses.

[8]  S. Nordin,et al.  Lateralization of olfactory cognitive functions: effects of rhinal side of stimulation. , 2001, Chemical senses.

[9]  R. Stevenson,et al.  Differences in naming accuracy of odors presented to the left and right nostrils , 2001, Biological Psychology.

[10]  D. Wilson,et al.  Receptive fields in the rat piriform cortex. , 2001, Chemical senses.

[11]  I Savic,et al.  Right-nostril dominance in discrimination of unfamiliar, but not familiar, odours. , 2000, Chemical senses.

[12]  I Savic,et al.  PET shows that odors are processed both ipsilaterally and contralaterally to the stimulated nostril , 2000, Neuroreport.

[13]  L. Cahill,et al.  Hemispheric lateralization in the processing of odor pleasantness versus odor names. , 1999, Chemical senses.

[14]  John D. E. Gabrieli,et al.  Olfaction: The world smells different to each nostril , 1999, Nature.

[15]  M. Laska,et al.  Odor identification, consistency of label use, olfactory threshold and their relationships to odor memory over the human lifespan. , 1999, Chemical senses.

[16]  Anthony R. McIntosh,et al.  Age-Related Changes in Regional Cerebral Blood Flow during Working Memory for Faces , 1998, NeuroImage.

[17]  M. Larsson,et al.  Semantic factors in episodic recognition of common odors in early and late adulthood: a review. , 1997, Chemical senses.

[18]  Mari Backman,et al.  Age-related differences in episodic odour recognition: the role of access to specific odour names. , 1997 .

[19]  R. Doty,et al.  Odor Recognition Memory Is Better Under Bilateral Than Unilateral Test Conditions , 1995, Cortex.

[20]  R. Zatorre,et al.  Right-nostril advantage for discrimination of odors , 1990, Perception & psychophysics.

[21]  Mark A. McDaniel,et al.  Memory for odors and odor names : Modalities of elaboration and imagery , 1990 .

[22]  M. T. Shipley,et al.  the connections of the mouse olfactory bulb: A study using orthograde and retrograde transport of wheat germ agglutinin conjugated to horseradish peroxidase , 1984, Brain Research Bulletin.

[23]  W S Cain,et al.  Odor recognition: familiarity, identifiability, and encoding consistency. , 1984, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[24]  B M Ross,et al.  Long-term memory of odors with and without verbal descriptions. , 1973, Journal of experimental psychology.

[25]  P D Eimas,et al.  Short-term memory of odors. , 1973, Journal of experimental psychology.

[26]  J. Fahrenkrug,et al.  Nasal swell-bodies and cyclic changes in the air passage of the rat and rabbit nose. , 1971, Journal of anatomy.

[27]  R. Sperry,et al.  Lateralization of olfactory perception in the surgically separated hemispheres of man , 1969 .

[28]  A. Baddeley Working Memory, Thought, and Action , 2007 .

[29]  Diana Caine Ba BSc Ma Memory from A to Z: Keywords, Concepts and Beyond , 2005 .

[30]  M. Treisman,et al.  Phonological and perceptual components of short-term memory for odors. , 1998, The American journal of psychology.

[31]  A. Holley,et al.  Role of verbal encoding in shortand long-term odor recognition , 1997, Perception & psychophysics.

[32]  L. Bäckman,et al.  Age-related differences in episodic odour recognition: the role of access to specific odour names. , 1997, Memory.

[33]  W. Cain,et al.  Sensory and semantic factors in recognition memory for odors and graphic stimuli: elderly versus young persons. , 1991, The American journal of psychology.

[34]  T. Finger,et al.  Neurobiology of taste and smell , 1987 .