Evidence for a dynamic-dominance hypothesis of handedness

Abstract. Handedness is a prominent behavioral phenomenon that emerges from asymmetrical neural organization of human motor systems. However, the aspects of motor performance that correspond to handedness remain largely undetermined. A recent study examining interlimb differences in coordination of reaching demonstrated dominant arm advantages in controlling limb segment inertial dynamics (Sainburg and Kalakanis 2000). Based on these findings, I now propose the dynamic-dominance hypothesis, which states that the essential factor that distinguishes dominant from nondominant arm performance is the facility governing the control of limb dynamics. The purpose of this study is to test two predictions of this hypothesis: 1) adaptation to novel intersegmental dynamics, requiring the development of new dynamic transforms, should be more effective for the dominant arm; 2) there should be no difference in adapting to visuomotor rotations performed with the dominant as compared with the nondominant arm. The latter prediction is based on the idea that visual information about target position is translated into an internal reference frame prior to transformation of the movement plan into dynamic properties, which reflect the forces required to produce movement. To test these predictions, dominant arm adaptation is compared to nondominant arm adaptation during exposure to novel inertial loads and to novel visuomotor rotations. The results indicate substantial interlimb differences in adaptation to novel inertial dynamics, but equivalent adaptation to novel visuomotor rotations. Inverse dynamic analysis revealed better coordination of dominant arm muscle torques across both shoulder and elbow joints, as compared with nondominant arm muscle torques. As a result, dominant arm movements were produced with a fraction of the mean squared muscle torque computed for nondominant arm movements made at similar speeds. These results support the dynamic-dominance hypothesis, indicating that interlimb asymmetries in control arise downstream to visuomotor transformations, when dynamic variables that correspond to the forces required for motion are specified.

[1]  Robert Sessions Woodworth,et al.  THE ACCURACY OF VOLUNTARY MOVEMENT , 1899 .

[2]  H. Liepmann,et al.  Die linke Hemisphäre und das Handeln.1) , 1908 .

[3]  B. C. Abbott,et al.  The relation between velocity of shortening and the tension‐length curve of skeletal muscle , 1953, The Journal of physiology.

[4]  P. Fitts The information capacity of the human motor system in controlling the amplitude of movement. , 1954, Journal of experimental psychology.

[5]  D. Wilkie The mechanical properties of muscle. , 1956, British medical bulletin.

[6]  H. Kuypers,et al.  Subcorticospinal projections in the Rhesus monkey , 1962, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[7]  P. Fitts,et al.  Information capacity of discrete motor responses under different cognitive sets. , 1966, Journal of experimental psychology.

[8]  P. Fitts Cognitive aspects of information processing. 3. Set for speed versus accuracy. , 1966, Journal of experimental psychology.

[9]  D. G. Lawrence,et al.  The functional organization of the motor system in the monkey. I. The effects of bilateral pyramidal lesions. , 1968, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[10]  D. G. Lawrence,et al.  The functional organization of the motor system in the monkey. II. The effects of lesions of the descending brain-stem pathways. , 1968, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[11]  M. Posner,et al.  Processing of visual feedback in rapid movements. , 1968, Journal of experimental psychology.

[12]  R. Schmidt Movement time as a determiner of timing accuracy. , 1969, Journal of experimental psychology.

[13]  H. Kuypers,et al.  Precentral projections to different parts of the spinal intermediate zone in therhesus monkey. , 1970, Brain research.

[14]  G Rizzolatti,et al.  Simple reaction times of ipsilateral and contralateral hand to lateralized visual stimuli. , 1971, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[15]  R. C. Oldfield The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. , 1971, Neuropsychologia.

[16]  R A Schmidt,et al.  Movement velocity and movement time as determiners of degree of preprogramming in simple movements. , 1972, Journal of experimental psychology.

[17]  J. Baily,et al.  Adaptation to Prisms: Do Proprioceptive Changes Mediate Adapted Behaviour with Ballistic Arm Movements? , 1972, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[18]  E Donchin,et al.  Studies of Squeezing: Handedness, Responding Hand, Response Force, and Asymmetry of Readiness Potential , 1974, Science.

[19]  H. Kuypers,et al.  Retrograde axonal transport of horseradish peroxidase from spinal cord to brain stem cell groups in the cat , 1975, Neuroscience Letters.

[20]  N. Geschwind The apraxias: neural mechanisms of disorders of learned movement. , 1975, American scientist.

[21]  K. Flowers,et al.  Handedness and controlled movement. , 1975, British journal of psychology.

[22]  K. Haaland,et al.  Motor performance after unilateral hemisphere damage in patients with tumor. , 1977, Archives of neurology.

[23]  J. Annett,et al.  The Control of Movement in the Preferred and Non-Preferred Hands* , 1979, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[24]  Kenneth M. Heilman,et al.  Left-Hemisphere Motor Dominance in Righthandersi , 1980, Cortex.

[25]  J I Todor,et al.  Hand differences in the rate and variability of rapid tapping. , 1980, Journal of motor behavior.

[26]  K M Heilman,et al.  Left-hemisphere motor dominance in righthanders. , 1980, Cortex; a journal devoted to the study of the nervous system and behavior.

[27]  D. Rosenbaum Human movement initiation: specification of arm, direction, and extent. , 1980, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[28]  I. Hamada,et al.  Characteristics of the ipsilateral movement-related neuron in the motor cortex of the monkey , 1981, Brain Research.

[29]  Harold D. Delaney,et al.  Motor deficits after left or right hemisphere damage due to stroke or tumor , 1981, Neuropsychologia.

[30]  Eric A Roy,et al.  Interlimb Transfer after Adaptation to Visual Displacement: Patterns Predicted from the Functional Closeness of Limb Neural Control Centres , 1981, Perception.

[31]  H. Kuypers,et al.  Brain stem projections to spinal motoneuronal cell groups in rat studied by means of electron microscopy autoradiography. , 1982, Progress in brain research.

[32]  G. Holstege,et al.  The anatomy of brain stem pathways to the spinal cord in cat. A labeled amino acid tracing study. , 1982, Progress in brain research.

[33]  E. Bizzi,et al.  Human arm trajectory formation. , 1982, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[34]  H. Kuypers A new look at the organization of the motor system. , 1982, Progress in brain research.

[35]  S. A. Wallace,et al.  Visual Control of Discrete Aiming Movements , 1983, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[36]  M. Moscovitch,et al.  Intermanual information transfer in patients with lesions in the trunk of the corpus callosum , 1984, Neuropsychologia.

[37]  E. Bizzi,et al.  Posture control and trajectory formation during arm movement , 1984, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[38]  J I Todor,et al.  Accommodation to increased accuracy demands by the right and left hands. , 1985, Journal of motor behavior.

[39]  T. Flash,et al.  The coordination of arm movements: an experimentally confirmed mathematical model , 1985, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[40]  Bilateral Transfer across Ages 7 to 17 Years , 1986, Perceptual and motor skills.

[41]  N. Geschwind,et al.  Handedness is not a Unidimensional Trait , 1986, Cortex.

[42]  D. Elliott,et al.  Manual asymmetries in visually directed aiming. , 1986, Canadian journal of psychology.

[43]  A. Smiley-Oyen,et al.  Force modulation as a source of hand differences in rapid finger tapping , 1987 .

[44]  H. Kuypers,et al.  Brainstem projections to lumbar motoneurons in rat—I. An ultrastructural study using autoradiography and the combination of autoradiography and horseradish peroxidase histochemistry , 1987, Neuroscience.

[45]  E Bizzi Motor control mechanisms. An overview. , 1987, Neurologic clinics.

[46]  Deborah L. Harrington,et al.  The effects of task complexity on motor performance in left and right CVA patients , 1987, Neuropsychologia.

[47]  J. Tanji,et al.  Neuronal activity in cortical motor areas related to ipsilateral, contralateral, and bilateral digit movements of the monkey. , 1988, Journal of neurophysiology.

[48]  宇野 洋二,et al.  Formation and control of optimal trajectory in human multijoint arm movement : minimum torque-change model , 1988 .

[49]  Marcel Kinsbourne,et al.  Asymmetrical transfer of training between hands: Implications for interhemispheric communication in normal brain , 1989, Brain and Cognition.

[50]  F. Zajac Muscle and tendon: properties, models, scaling, and application to biomechanics and motor control. , 1989, Critical reviews in biomedical engineering.

[51]  D. Harrington,et al.  The role of the hemispheres in closed loop movements , 1989, Brain and Cognition.

[52]  Deborah L. Harrington,et al.  Hemispheric control of the initial and corrective components of aiming movements , 1989, Neuropsychologia.

[53]  C Ghez,et al.  Gradual specification of response amplitude in human tracking performance. , 1989, Brain, behavior and evolution.

[54]  Marcel Kinsbourne,et al.  Asymmetrical transfer of braille acquisition between hands , 1990, Brain and Language.

[55]  David A. Winter,et al.  Biomechanics and Motor Control of Human Movement , 1990 .

[56]  D. Elliott,et al.  The contribution of vision to asymmetries in manual aiming , 1990, Neuropsychologia.

[57]  John M. Hollerbach,et al.  Fundamentals of motor behavior , 1990 .

[58]  James Gordon,et al.  Organization of voluntary movement , 1991, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[59]  R. Nicoletti,et al.  Is interhemispheric transfer of visuomotor information asymmetric? Evidence from a meta-analysis , 1991, Neuropsychologia.

[60]  D. Dewey,et al.  The temporal locus of transfer of training between hands: an interference study , 1991, Behavioural Brain Research.

[61]  B. Shahani,et al.  Hemispheric threshold differences for motor evoked potentials produced by magnetic coil stimulation , 1991, Neurology.

[62]  M. Goodale,et al.  Separate visual pathways for perception and action , 1992, Trends in Neurosciences.

[63]  Digby Elliott,et al.  Asymmetries in the discrete and pseudocontinuous regulation of visually guided reaching , 1992, Brain and Cognition.

[64]  Michael I. Jordan,et al.  Forward Models: Supervised Learning with a Distal Teacher , 1992, Cogn. Sci..

[65]  P. Fitts The information capacity of the human motor system in controlling the amplitude of movement. 1954. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[66]  D. Elliott,et al.  Asymmetries in the Preparation and Control of Manual Aiming Movements , 1993 .

[67]  Richard G. Carson,et al.  Manual asymmetries: old problems and new directions , 1993 .

[68]  C. Ghez,et al.  Loss of proprioception produces deficits in interjoint coordination. , 1993, Journal of neurophysiology.

[69]  A. P. Georgopoulos,et al.  Functional magnetic resonance imaging of motor cortex: hemispheric asymmetry and handedness. , 1993, Science.

[70]  S. Kinomura,et al.  Regional cerebral blood flow changes of cortical motor areas and prefrontal areas in humans related to ipsilateral and contralateral hand movement , 1993, Brain Research.

[71]  F A Mussa-Ivaldi,et al.  Adaptive representation of dynamics during learning of a motor task , 1994, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[72]  J. Lackner,et al.  Rapid adaptation to Coriolis force perturbations of arm trajectory. , 1994, Journal of neurophysiology.

[73]  C. Winstein,et al.  Effects of physical guidance and knowledge of results on motor learning: support for the guidance hypothesis. , 1994, Research quarterly for exercise and sport.

[74]  D. Harrington,et al.  Limb-Sequencing Deficits After Left but not Right Hemisphere Damage , 1994, Brain and Cognition.

[75]  D. Elliott,et al.  The influence of intermittent vision on manual aiming. , 1994, Acta psychologica.

[76]  D. Elliott,et al.  Kinematic Analyses of Manual Asymmetries in Visual Aiming Movements , 1994, Brain and Cognition.

[77]  P. Roland,et al.  Activity in the human primary motor cortex related to ipsilateral hand movements , 1994, Brain Research.

[78]  D. Elliott,et al.  The Preparation of Aiming Movements , 1995, Brain and Cognition.

[79]  H. Imamizu,et al.  The locus of visual-motor learning at the task or manipulator level: implications from intermanual transfer. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[80]  M. Kawato,et al.  Internal representations of the motor apparatus: implications from generalization in visuomotor learning. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[81]  R Salmelin,et al.  Bilateral activation of the human somatomotor cortex by distal hand movements. , 1995, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[82]  R L Sainburg,et al.  Control of limb dynamics in normal subjects and patients without proprioception. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.

[83]  J. Gordon,et al.  Learning a visuomotor transformation in a local area of work space produces directional biases in other areas. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.

[84]  Michael I. Jordan,et al.  An internal model for sensorimotor integration. , 1995, Science.

[85]  Romeo Chua,et al.  The Influence of Target Perturbation on Manual Aiming Asymmetries in Right-Handers , 1995, Cortex.

[86]  T. Flash,et al.  Minimum-jerk, two-thirds power law, and isochrony: converging approaches to movement planning. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[87]  Tracy L. Faber,et al.  Role of posterior parietal cortex in the recalibration of visually guided reaching , 1996, Nature.

[88]  S. Kosslyn,et al.  Functional imaging of human right hemispheric activation for exploratory movements , 1996, Annals of neurology.

[89]  Michael I. Jordan,et al.  Generalization to Local Remappings of the Visuomotor Coordinate Transformation , 1996, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[90]  E Bizzi,et al.  Motor learning by field approximation. , 1996, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[91]  Intermanual transfer of somaesthetic information: A two-point discrimination experiment , 1996, Neuropsychologia.

[92]  G. Gottlieb,et al.  Coordinating movement at two joints: a principle of linear covariance. , 1996, Journal of neurophysiology.

[93]  P. Roland,et al.  Activation of Multi‐modal Cortical Areas Underlies Short‐term Memory , 1996, The European journal of neuroscience.

[94]  E. Bizzi,et al.  Consolidation in human motor memory , 1996, Nature.

[95]  A Urbano,et al.  Human cortical activity related to unilateral movements. A high resolution EEG study , 1996, Neuroreport.

[96]  D. Harrington,et al.  Hemispheric asymmetry of movement , 1996, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[97]  R. Kawashima,et al.  Changes in regional cerebral blood flow during self-paced arm and finger movements. A PET study , 1996, Brain Research.

[98]  D Bourbonnais,et al.  Performance of the 'unaffected' upper extremity of elderly stroke patients. , 1996, Stroke.

[99]  R. Shadmehr,et al.  Neural correlates of motor memory consolidation. , 1997, Science.

[100]  S G Kim,et al.  Functional activation in motor cortex reflects the direction and the degree of handedness. , 1997, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[101]  M. Hallett,et al.  Role of the Ipsilateral Motor Cortex in Voluntary Movement , 1997, Canadian Journal of Neurological Sciences / Journal Canadien des Sciences Neurologiques.

[102]  M. Hallett,et al.  Involvement of the ipsilateral motor cortex in finger movements of different complexities , 1997, Annals of neurology.

[103]  R Kawashima,et al.  Functional asymmetry of cortical motor control in left‐handed subjects , 1997, Neuroreport.

[104]  Audrey Chen,et al.  Brain asymmetry and facial attractiveness: Facial beauty is not simply in the eye of the beholder , 1997, Neuropsychologia.

[105]  T. Brashers-Krug,et al.  Functional Stages in the Formation of Human Long-Term Motor Memory , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[106]  Zoubin Ghahramani,et al.  Modular decomposition in visuomotor learning , 1997, Nature.

[107]  A Schnitzler,et al.  Handedness and asymmetry of hand representation in human motor cortex. , 1998, Journal of neurophysiology.

[108]  D. Cheyne,et al.  Neuromagnetic fields preceding unilateral movements in dextrals and sinistrals , 1998, Neuroreport.

[109]  Valentino Bettinardi,et al.  Hemispheric asymmetries and bimanual asynchrony in left- and right-handers , 1998, Experimental Brain Research.

[110]  D. Wolpert,et al.  Temporal and amplitude generalization in motor learning. , 1998, Journal of neurophysiology.

[111]  N. Sadato,et al.  Regional cerebral blood flow changes in human brain related to ipsilateral and contralateral complex hand movements – a PET study , 1998, The European journal of neuroscience.

[112]  M. Kawato,et al.  Adaptive internal model of intrinsic kinematics involved in learning an aiming task. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[113]  Mitsuo Kawato,et al.  Internal models for motor control and trajectory planning , 1999, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[114]  C. Winstein,et al.  Motor learning after unilateral brain damage , 1999, Neuropsychologia.

[115]  Y Uno,et al.  Quantitative examinations of internal representations for arm trajectory planning: minimum commanded torque change model. , 1999, Journal of neurophysiology.

[116]  Reza Shadmehr,et al.  Computational nature of human adaptive control during learning of reaching movements in force fields , 1999, Biological Cybernetics.

[117]  John W. Krakauer,et al.  Independent learning of internal models for kinematic and dynamic control of reaching , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.

[118]  R L Sainburg,et al.  Intersegmental dynamics are controlled by sequential anticipatory, error correction, and postural mechanisms. , 1999, Journal of neurophysiology.

[119]  R Shadmehr,et al.  Spatial Generalization from Learning Dynamics of Reaching Movements , 2000, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[120]  Zoubin Ghahramani,et al.  Computational principles of movement neuroscience , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[121]  C Ghez,et al.  Learning of Visuomotor Transformations for Vectorial Planning of Reaching Trajectories , 2000, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[122]  R A Scheidt,et al.  Persistence of motor adaptation during constrained, multi-joint, arm movements. , 2000, Journal of neurophysiology.

[123]  M. Ghilardi,et al.  Patterns of regional brain activation associated with different forms of motor learning , 2000, Brain Research.

[124]  R. Sainburg,et al.  Differences in control of limb dynamics during dominant and nondominant arm reaching. , 2000, Journal of neurophysiology.

[125]  D A Rosenbaum,et al.  Frames of reference in perceptual-motor learning: Evidence from a blind manual positioning task , 2001, Psychological research.