Lateralization of Executive Function: Working Memory Advantage for Same Hemifield Stimuli in the Monkey

Working memory capacity, the amount of information that may be maintained in mind over a period of seconds, is extremely limited, to a handful of items. Some evidence exists that the number of visual items that may be maintained in working memory is independent for the two hemifields. To test this idea, we trained monkeys to perform visual working memory tasks that required maintenance in memory of the locations and/or shapes of 3–5 visual stimuli. We then tested whether systematic performance differences were present for stimuli concentrated in the same hemifield, vs. distributed across hemifields. We found little evidence to support the expectation that working memory capacity is independent in the two hemifields. Instead, when an advantage of stimulus arrangement was present, it involved multiple stimuli presented in the same hemifield. This conclusion was consistent across variations of the task, performance levels, and apparent strategies adopted by individual subjects. This result suggests that factors such as grouping that favor processing of stimuli in relative proximity may counteract the benefits of independent processing in the two hemispheres. Our results reveal an important property of working memory and place constraints on models of working memory capacity.

[1]  B. Richmond,et al.  Humans and monkeys use different strategies to solve the same short-term memory tasks , 2016, Learning & memory.

[2]  M. Stokes ‘Activity-silent’ working memory in prefrontal cortex: a dynamic coding framework , 2015, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[3]  Markus Siegel,et al.  Neural substrates of cognitive capacity limitations , 2011, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[4]  M. Banich The Missing Link: The Role of Interhemispheric Interaction in Attentional Processing , 1998, Brain and Cognition.

[5]  Evelien Heyselaar,et al.  A change detection approach to study visual working memory of the macaque monkey. , 2011, Journal of vision.

[6]  G. Humphreys,et al.  Bilateral Field Advantage in Visual Enumeration , 2011, PLoS ONE.

[7]  D. C. Essen,et al.  The topographic organization of rhesus monkey prestriate cortex. , 1978, The Journal of physiology.

[8]  M. Alpert LATERALIZATION OF THE HUMAN BRAIN , 1977, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[9]  Angela D. Friederici,et al.  The ontogeny of the cortical language network , 2016, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[10]  Jesper Tegnér,et al.  Mechanism for top-down control of working memory capacity , 2009, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[11]  Yaoda Xu,et al.  Visual grouping in human parietal cortex , 2007, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[12]  Patrick Cavanagh,et al.  Quadrantic deficit reveals anatomical constraints on selection , 2007, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[13]  Christos Constantinidis,et al.  Comparison of Neural Activity Related to Working Memory in Primate Dorsolateral Prefrontal and Posterior Parietal Cortex , 2010, Front. Syst. Neurosci..

[14]  Matthew L. Leavitt,et al.  A Quadrantic Bias in Prefrontal Representation of Visual-Mnemonic Space , 2018, Cerebral cortex.

[15]  P. Cavanagh,et al.  Independent Resources for Attentional Tracking in the Left and Right Visual Hemifields , 2005, Psychological science.

[16]  Dwight J. Peterson,et al.  Contralateral delay activity tracks the influence of Gestalt grouping principles on active visual working memory representations , 2015, Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics.

[17]  Yaoda Xu Understanding the object benefit in visual short-term memory: The roles of feature proximity and connectedness , 2006, Perception & Psychophysics.

[18]  Dwight J. Peterson,et al.  The Gestalt principle of similarity benefits visual working memory , 2013, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[19]  Ryan A. Rivera,et al.  A software solution for the control, acquisition, and storage of CAPTAN network topologies , 2008, 2008 IEEE Nuclear Science Symposium Conference Record.

[20]  A. Compte,et al.  Bump attractor dynamics in prefrontal cortex explains behavioral precision in spatial working memory , 2014, Nature Neuroscience.

[21]  C. Constantinidis,et al.  The neuroscience of working memory capacity and training , 2016, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[22]  Edward Awh,et al.  A bilateral advantage for storage in visual working memory , 2010, Cognition.

[23]  V. Mountcastle,et al.  Posterior parietal association cortex of the monkey: command functions for operations within extrapersonal space. , 1975, Journal of neurophysiology.

[24]  M. Banich,et al.  An unbalanced distribution of inputs across the hemispheres facilitates interhemispheric interaction , 2000, Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society.

[25]  Yuhong Jiang,et al.  The unilateral field advantage in repetition detection: Effects of perceptual grouping and task demands , 2010, Attention, perception & psychophysics.

[26]  Nava Rubin,et al.  Perceptual Completion across the Vertical Meridian and the Role of Early Visual Cortex , 2002, Neuron.

[27]  Earl K Miller,et al.  The representation of multiple objects in prefrontal neuronal delay activity. , 2007, Cerebral cortex.

[28]  John Duncan,et al.  Spatial and temporal distribution of visual information coding in lateral prefrontal cortex , 2014, The European journal of neuroscience.

[29]  C. Constantinidis,et al.  Persistent discharges in the prefrontal cortex of monkeys naive to working memory tasks. , 2007, Cerebral cortex.

[30]  P. Cavanagh,et al.  A unilateral field advantage for detecting repeated elements , 2008, Perception & psychophysics.

[31]  M. R. Riley,et al.  Role of Prefrontal Persistent Activity in Working Memory , 2016, Front. Syst. Neurosci..

[32]  Ayano Matsushima,et al.  Different Neuronal Computations of Spatial Working Memory for Multiple Locations within versus across Visual Hemifields , 2014, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[33]  M. Chun,et al.  Organization of visual short-term memory. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[34]  P. Cavanagh,et al.  Anatomical constraints on attention: hemifield independence is a signature of multifocal spatial selection. , 2012, Journal of vision.

[35]  Daniel R. Little,et al.  Hemifield Effects in Multiple Identity Tracking , 2012, PloS one.

[36]  P. Goldman-Rakic,et al.  Synaptic mechanisms and network dynamics underlying spatial working memory in a cortical network model. , 2000, Cerebral cortex.

[37]  Patrick Cavanagh,et al.  Within-Hemifield Competition in Early Visual Areas Limits the Ability to Track Multiple Objects with Attention , 2014, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[38]  J. Wallis,et al.  Capacity and precision in an animal model of visual short-term memory. , 2012, Journal of vision.

[39]  D H Brainard,et al.  The Psychophysics Toolbox. , 1997, Spatial vision.

[40]  T. Stanford,et al.  Stimulus Selectivity in Dorsal and Ventral Prefrontal Cortex after Training in Working Memory Tasks , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[41]  M. Banich Integration of Information Between the Cerebral Hemispheres , 1998 .

[42]  C. Braun,et al.  Intra and inter hemispheric dynamics revealed by reaction time in the Dimond paradigm: A quantitative review of the literature , 2014, Neuropsychologia.

[43]  An-Shik Yang,et al.  Investigation of piezoelectrically generated synthetic jet flow , 2009, J. Vis..

[44]  Jean-François Delvenne,et al.  The capacity of visual short-term memory within and between hemifields , 2005, Cognition.

[45]  Christos Constantinidis,et al.  A software solution for the control of visual behavioral experimentation , 2005, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[46]  R. Desimone,et al.  Visual areas in the temporal cortex of the macaque , 1979, Brain Research.

[47]  R. Cabeza,et al.  Cross-Hemispheric Collaboration and Segregation Associated with Task Difficulty as Revealed by Structural and Functional Connectivity , 2015, The Journal of Neuroscience.