German children's productivity with tense morphology: the Perfekt (present perfect)

Two nonce-word studies examined German-speaking children's productivity with the Perfekt (present perfect) from 2;6 to 3;6. The German Perfekt consists of the past participle of the main verb and an inflected form of an auxiliary (either haben ‘have’ or sein ‘be’). In Study 1, nonce verbs were either introduced in the infinitival form, and children (seventy-two children, aged 2;6 to 3;6) were tested on their ability to produce the Perfekt, or introduced in the Perfekt, and children were tested on their ability to produce the infinitive. In Study 2 twenty-four children aged 3;6 were given the past participle form of nonce verbs to see if they could supply the appropriate auxiliary (based mainly on verb semantics). The results were that many children as young as 2;6 used past participles productively (more than used infinitival forms productively), but all children had much difficulty in supplying both auxiliaries appropriately. The current findings suggest that mastery of the Perfekt construction as a whole does not take place before the age of four and that frequency of exposure is an important factor in determining the age at which children acquire grammatical constructions.

[1]  B. MacWhinney The Acquisition Of Morphophonology , 1978 .

[2]  M. Pérez-Pereira The acquisition of morphemes: some evidence from Spanish. , 1989, Journal of psycholinguistic research.

[3]  H. Clahsen,et al.  Netzwerke und symbolische Regeln im Spracherwerb: experimentelle Ergebnisse zur Entwicklung der Flexionsmorphologie , 1994 .

[4]  Max H. Miller,et al.  Zur Logik der frühkindlichen Sprachentwicklung : empirische Untersuchungen und Theoriediskussion , 1976 .

[5]  The Child's Conception of Time. Le Developpement de la Notion du Temps Chez l 'Enfant , 1971 .

[6]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Young children's productivity with word order and verb morphology. , 1997, Developmental psychology.

[7]  Wolfgang Klein,et al.  The acquisition of German , 1992 .

[8]  R. Cann TIME, TENSE AND ASPECT , 1993 .

[9]  F. Antinucci,et al.  How children talk about what happened , 1976, Journal of Child Language.

[10]  L. Talmy Lexicalisation patterns: semantic structure in lexical forms , 1985 .

[11]  R. Weist,et al.  The defective tense hypothesis: on the emergence of tense and aspect in child Polish , 1984, Journal of Child Language.

[12]  Gerhard Helbig,et al.  Deutsche Grammatik : ein Handbuch für den Ausländerunterricht , 2001 .

[13]  S Pinker,et al.  Overregularization in language acquisition. , 1992, Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development.

[14]  V. Marchman Children's productivity in the English past tense: The role of frequency, phonology, and neighborhoo , 1997 .

[15]  M. Tomasello Do young children have adult syntactic competence? , 2000, Cognition.

[16]  The unaccusative hypothesis and the history of the perfect auxiliary in Germanic and Romance , 1990 .

[17]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Twenty-Five-Month-Old Children Do Not Have a Grammatical Category of Verb. , 1993 .

[18]  MICHAEL TOMASELLO,et al.  Young children's earliest transitive and intransitive constructions , 1998 .

[19]  R. Baayen,et al.  Acquiring unaccusativity: a cross-linguistic look , 2004 .

[20]  Harald Clahsen,et al.  The representation of participles in the German mental lexicon: Evidence for the dual-mechanism model , 1997 .

[21]  J. Berko The Child's Learning of English Morphology , 1958 .

[22]  B. MacWhinney,et al.  The Crosslinguistic Study of Sentence Processing. , 1992 .

[23]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Differential productivity in young children's use of nouns and verbs , 1997, Journal of Child Language.

[24]  L. Bloom,et al.  Semantics of verbs and the development of verb inflection in child language , 1980 .

[25]  B. MacWhinney,et al.  Functionalism and the competition model , 1989 .

[26]  Virginia A. Marchman,et al.  Children's Productivity in the English Past Tense: The Role of Frequency, Phonology, and Neighborhood Structure , 1997, Cogn. Sci..