Memory and foraging theory: chimpanzee utilization of optimality heuristics in the rank-order recovery of hidden foods

Many models from foraging theory and movement ecology assume that resources are encountered randomly. If food locations, types and values are retained in memory, however, search time could be significantly reduced, with concurrent effects on biological fitness. Despite this, little is known about what specific characteristics of foods, particularly those relevant to profitability, nonhuman animals can remember. Building upon previous observations, we hypothesized that chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes), after observing foods being hidden in a large wooded test area they could not enter, and after long delays, would direct (through gesture and vocalization) experimentally naïve humans to the reward locations in an order that could be predicted beforehand by the spatial and physical characteristics of those items. In the main experiment, various quantities of almonds, both in and out of shells and sealed in transparent bags, were hidden in the test area. The chimpanzees later directed searchers to those items in a nonrandom order related to quantity, shell presence/absence, and the distance they were hidden from the subject. The recovery sequences were closely related to the actual e/h profitability of the foods. Predicted recovery orders, based on the energetic value of almonds and independently-measured, individual-specific expected pursuit and processing times, were closely related to observed recovery orders. We argue that the information nonhuman animals possess regarding their environment can be extensive, and that further comparative study is vital for incorporating realistic cognitive variables into models of foraging and movement.

[1]  Lovejoy,et al.  The Chimpanzee Has No Clothes: A Critical Examination of Pan troglodytes in Models of Human Evolution [with Comments] , 2008 .

[2]  Responses to quantity: Perceptual versus cognitive mechanisms in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). , 1995 .

[3]  Christophe Boesch,et al.  Sophisticated Euclidean maps in forest chimpanzees , 2009, Animal Behaviour.

[4]  D. Kahneman,et al.  Attention and Effort , 1973 .

[5]  V. Křivan,et al.  A mechanistic model for partial preferences. , 2000, Theoretical population biology.

[6]  Richard W. Byrne,et al.  Travel routes and planning of visits to out-of-sight resources in wild chacma baboons, Papio ursinus , 2007, Animal Behaviour.

[7]  R. Byrne,et al.  Foraging behaviour in domestic pigs (Sus scrofa): remembering and prioritizing food sites of different value , 2005, Animal Cognition.

[8]  W. Köhler The Mentality of Apes. , 2018, Nature.

[9]  M. Hauser,et al.  The effect of handling time on temporal discounting in two New World primates , 2006, Animal Behaviour.

[10]  S. Mizumori,et al.  Neurons in rat medial prefrontal cortex show anticipatory rate changes to predictable differential rewards in a spatial memory task , 2001, Behavioural Brain Research.

[11]  Burt P. Kotler,et al.  Short-Term Apparent Competition , 1987, The American Naturalist.

[12]  P. Garber,et al.  Role of spatial memory in primate foraging patterns: Saguinus mystax and Saguinus fuscicollis , 1989, American journal of primatology.

[13]  Effects of energy costs on the optimal diet: an experiment with pied flycatchers Ficedula hypoleuca feeding nestlings , 1988 .

[14]  A. Dickinson,et al.  Episodic-like memory during cache recovery by scrub jays , 1998, Nature.

[15]  William A. Mitchell,et al.  An optimal control theory of diet selection : the effects of resource depletion and exploitative competition , 1990 .

[16]  S. Shettleworth Cognition, evolution, and behavior , 1998 .

[17]  O. L. Tinklepaugh The multiple delayed reaction with chimpanzees and monkeys. , 1932 .

[18]  M. Manser,et al.  Spatial representation of shelter locations in meerkats, Suricata suricatta , 2004, Animal Behaviour.

[19]  M. Beran,et al.  Perception of food amounts by chimpanzees based on the number, size, contour length and visibility of items , 2008, Animal Behaviour.

[20]  W. J. Conover,et al.  Practical Nonparametric Statistics , 1972 .

[21]  N. Heglund,et al.  Energetics and mechanics of terrestrial locomotion. I. Metabolic energy consumption as a function of speed and body size in birds and mammals. , 1982, The Journal of experimental biology.

[22]  Germinal Cocho,et al.  Scale-free foraging by primates emerges from their interaction with a complex environment , 2006, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[23]  K. Janmaat,et al.  Exploring New Areas: How Important is Long-Term Spatial Memory for Mangabey (Lophocebus albigena johnstonii) Foraging Efficiency? , 2010, International Journal of Primatology.

[24]  A. Dickinson,et al.  Memory for the content of caches by scrub jays (Aphelocoma coerulescens). , 1999, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[25]  C. Janson,et al.  Integrating information about location and value of resources by white-faced saki monkeys (Pithecia pithecia) , 2007, Animal Cognition.

[26]  Charles R. Menzel Machiavellian Intelligence II: Primates' knowledge of their natural habitat: As indicated in foraging , 1997 .

[27]  Stephen Beckerman,et al.  Optimal Foraging and Hominid Evolution: Labor and Reciprocity , 1985 .

[28]  K. Waddington,et al.  Optimal Foraging: On Flower Selection by Bees , 1979, The American Naturalist.

[29]  D. H. Morse,et al.  Stochastic prey arrivals and crab spider giving-up times: simulations of spider performance using two simple “rules of thumb” , 1989, Oecologia.

[30]  Nils Chr. Stenseth,et al.  A general version of optimal foraging theory: The effect of simultaneous encounters , 1984 .

[31]  C. Menzel,et al.  Unprompted recall and reporting of hidden objects by a chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) after extended delays. , 1999, Journal of comparative psychology.

[32]  Charles H. Janson,et al.  Experimental evidence for route integration and strategic planning in wild capuchin monkeys , 2007, Animal Cognition.

[33]  C. Menzel,et al.  Spontaneous use of matching visual cues during foraging by long-tailed macaques (Macaca fascicularis). , 1996, Journal of comparative psychology.

[34]  B. Cole,et al.  Imperfectly optimal animals , 1981, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.

[35]  E W Menzel,et al.  Selection of Food by Size in the Chimpanzee, and Comparison with Human Judgments , 1960, Science.

[36]  K. Hawkes,et al.  why hunters gather: optimal foraging and the Aché of eastern Paraguay , 1982 .

[37]  F. Bartumeus,et al.  Optimal search behavior and classic foraging theory , 2009 .

[38]  David A. Washburn,et al.  Intelligence of Apes and Other Rational Beings , 2003 .

[39]  Cresswell,et al.  The Effects of a Limited Memory Capacity on Foraging Behavior , 1997, Theoretical population biology.

[40]  C. Lovejoy,et al.  The Chimpanzee Has No Clothes , 2008, Current Anthropology.

[41]  Richard Byrne,et al.  What wild primates know about resources: opening up the black box , 2007, Animal Cognition.

[42]  C. Janson Experimental evidence for spatial memory in foraging wild capuchin monkeys, Cebus apella , 1998, Animal Behaviour.

[43]  S. Shettleworth,et al.  How pigeons estimate rates of prey encounter. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[44]  M. Lynch,et al.  Long-term potentiation and memory. , 2004, Physiological reviews.

[45]  F. Barraquand,et al.  Cognitive abilities of a central place forager interact with prey spatial aggregation in their effect on intake rate , 2009, Animal Behaviour.

[46]  M. Norconk,et al.  Optimal foraging on the roof of the world: Himalayan langurs and the classical prey model. , 2009, American journal of physical anthropology.

[47]  S. Pochron,et al.  Can Concurrent Speed and Directness of Travel Indicate Purposeful Encounter in the Yellow Baboons (Papio hamadryas cynocephalus) of Ruaha National Park, Tanzania? , 2001, International Journal of Primatology.

[48]  C. Menzel Progress in the Study of Chimpanzee Recall and Episodic Memory. , 2005 .

[49]  Bruce Winterhalder,et al.  Hunter-gatherer foraging strategies : ethnographic and archeological analyses , 1983 .

[50]  R. Menzel,et al.  Honey bees navigate according to a map-like spatial memory. , 2005, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[51]  R. Gray,et al.  Perceptual constraints on optimal foraging: The effects of variation among foragers , 1996, Evolutionary Ecology.

[52]  E. Menzel Chimpanzee Spatial Memory Organization , 1973, Science.

[53]  E. Menzel,et al.  Communication about the environment in a group of young chimpanzees. , 1971, Folia primatologica; international journal of primatology.

[54]  Sasha R. X. Dall,et al.  Information and its use by animals in evolutionary ecology. , 2005, Trends in ecology & evolution.

[55]  H. Larralde,et al.  Lévy walk patterns in the foraging movements of spider monkeys (Ateles geoffroyi) , 2003, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.

[56]  Do solitary foraging nocturnal mammals plan their routes? , 2011, Biology Letters.

[57]  William A. Mitchell,et al.  Diet selection on depletable resources , 1989 .

[58]  I. Tibbetts,et al.  Foraging behaviour and prey discrimination in the bluespotted maskray Dasyatis kuhlii , 2008 .

[59]  D. Sherry Food storage by black-capped chickadees: Memory for the location and contents of caches , 1984, Animal Behaviour.

[60]  C. Menzel Cognitive aspects of foraging in Japanese monkeys , 1991, Animal Behaviour.

[61]  S. Benhamou HOW MANY ANIMALS REALLY DO THE LÉVY WALK , 2007 .

[62]  S. Shettleworth Handling time and choice in pigeons. , 1985, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.