Ironic processes in the mental control of mood and mood-related thought.

The mental control of mood and mood-related thought was investigated. In Experiment 1, Ss reminiscing about a happy or sad event were asked to make their mood positive, were given no instructions, or were asked to make their mood negative. Ss attempting mood control without an imposed cognitive load were successful, whereas those who attempted control while rehearsing a 9-digit number not only failed to control their moods but also showed self-reported mood change opposite the mood they intended to create. In Experiment 2, Ss attempting to control mood-related thoughts under cognitive load showed increased accessibility of those thoughts contrary to the direction of intended control in a Stroop-type color-naming task.

[1]  Colin M. Macleod Half a century of research on the Stroop effect: an integrative review. , 1991, Psychological bulletin.

[2]  D. Wegner Ironic processes of mental control. , 1994, Psychological review.

[3]  A. Hochschild Emotion Work, Feeling Rules, and Social Structure , 1979, American Journal of Sociology.

[4]  Christopher K. Hsee,et al.  Emotional intelligence and the self-regulation of affect. , 1993 .

[5]  G. Logan Toward an instance theory of automatization. , 1988 .

[6]  E. Higgins,et al.  Communication, social cognition, and affect , 1988 .

[7]  Felicia Pratto,et al.  Individual construct accessibility and perceptual selection , 1986 .

[8]  Daniel M. Wegner,et al.  The hyperaccessibility of suppressed thoughts. , 1992 .

[9]  M. Posner,et al.  Attention and cognitive control. , 1975 .

[10]  J. Bargh Automatic and conscious processing of social information. , 1984 .

[11]  J. C. Masters,et al.  Children's strategies for the control of emotion in themselves and others. , 1990 .

[12]  W. T. Powers Behavior, the control of perception , 1973 .

[13]  Maryanne Martin On the induction of mood , 1990 .

[14]  J. Brehm A theory of psychological reactance. , 1981 .

[15]  C. Carver,et al.  Attention and Self-Regulation: A Control-Theory Approach to Human Behavior , 1981 .

[16]  J. Bargh Conditional automaticity: Varieties of automatic influence in social perception and cognition. , 1989 .

[17]  G. Miller,et al.  Plans and the structure of behavior , 1960 .

[18]  J. Uleman A framework for thinking intentionally about unintended thoughts , 1989 .

[19]  E Svensson,et al.  The measurement of mood. , 1979, Scandinavian journal of psychology.

[20]  Walter Mischel,et al.  Influence of affect on cognitive social learning person variables. , 1982 .

[21]  S. R. Strong,et al.  A Comparison of Paradoxical and Nonparadoxical Interpretations and Directives. , 1982 .

[22]  R. Solomon,et al.  An opponent-process theory of motivation. I. Temporal dynamics of affect. , 1974, Psychological review.

[23]  I. Gotlib,et al.  Construct accessibility and depression: an examination of cognitive and affective factors. , 1984, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[24]  Daniel M. Wegner White Bears and Other Unwanted Thoughts , 1990 .

[25]  J. Stroop Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. , 1992 .

[26]  S. Rachman,et al.  Experimental investigations of the relations between mood and intrusive unwanted cognitions. , 1982, The British journal of medical psychology.

[27]  E. Velten A laboratory task for induction of mood states. , 1968, Behaviour research and therapy.

[28]  D M Wegner,et al.  Depression and mental control: the resurgence of unwanted negative thoughts. , 1988, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[29]  S. Strong,et al.  Stimulating therapeutic change with interpretations: A comparison of positive and negative connotation. , 1982 .

[30]  G. Logan Attention and automaticity in Stroop and priming tasks: Theory and data , 1980, Cognitive Psychology.

[31]  L. Rehm A self-control model of depression , 1977 .

[32]  J. Bargh,et al.  The Role of Consciousness in Priming Effects on Categorization , 1987 .

[33]  Daniel T. Gilbert,et al.  Thinking backward: some curable and incurable consequences of cognitive busyness , 1989 .

[34]  J. Gross,et al.  Emotional suppression: physiology, self-report, and expressive behavior. , 1993, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[35]  G. Moskowitz Cognitive social psychology : the Princeton Symposium on the Legacy and Future of Social Cognition , 1981 .

[36]  Daniel M. Wegner,et al.  You can't always think what you want: Problems in the suppression of unwanted thoughts , 1992 .

[37]  G. A. Mendelsohn,et al.  Affect grid : A single-item scale of pleasure and arousal , 1989 .

[38]  L. Hasher,et al.  Automatic and effortful processes in memory. , 1979 .

[39]  G. Miller,et al.  Plans and the structure of behavior , 1960 .

[40]  Michael Conway,et al.  Negative affect leads to low-effort cognition: attributional processing for observed social behavior , 1989 .

[41]  S. Nolen-Hoeksema,et al.  Sex Differences in Unipolar Depression: Evidence and Theory Background on the Affective Disorders , 1987 .

[42]  A. Isen,et al.  Affect and Social Behavior. , 1991 .

[43]  V. Rippere "What's the thing to do when you're feeling depressed?" --a pilot study. , 1977, Behaviour research and therapy.

[44]  R. Wenzlaff The mental control of depression: Psychological obstacles to emotional well-being. , 1993 .

[45]  James W. Pennebaker,et al.  The Handbook of Mental Control , 1992 .

[46]  A. Tesser,et al.  Task effort and the regulation of mood: The absorption hypothesis , 1992 .

[47]  L. Wheeler,et al.  Review of personality and social psychology , 1980 .

[48]  David R. Paredes,et al.  Micromanaging time: Measuring and controlling timing errors in computer-controlled experiments , 1990 .

[49]  T. K. Srull,et al.  Handbook of Social Cognition , 1993 .

[50]  L. L. Martin,et al.  Set/reset: use and disuse of concepts in impression formation. , 1986, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[51]  E. Higgins,et al.  Do self-attributes form a cognitive structure? , 1988 .