Botallo's error, or the quandaries of the universality assumption

[1]  E. Bisiach,et al.  Unilateral Neglect of Representational Space , 1978, Cortex.

[2]  Tim Shallice,et al.  Case study approach in neuropsychological research , 1979 .

[3]  R. Rosenthal The file drawer problem and tolerance for null results , 1979 .

[4]  K. Heilman,et al.  Pseudoneglect: Effects of hemispace on a tactile line bisection task , 1980, Neuropsychologia.

[5]  D. Perani,et al.  Brain and conscious representation of outside reality , 1981, Neuropsychologia.

[6]  A. Caramazza On drawing inferences about the structure of normal cognitive systems from the analysis of patterns of impaired performance: The case for single-patient studies , 1986, Brain and Cognition.

[7]  T. Shallice From Neuropsychology to Mental Structure: Converging Operations: Specific Syndromes and Evidence from Normal Subjects , 1988 .

[8]  A. Caramazza,et al.  The case for single-patient studies , 1988 .

[9]  John C. Marshall,et al.  Idealisation meets psychometrics: The case for the right groups and the right individuals , 1988 .

[10]  K. L. Moore Meaning of “normal” , 1989 .

[11]  R. Logie,et al.  Case Studies in Working Memory: A Case for Single Cases? , 1991, Cortex.

[12]  M Imbert,et al.  Influence of reading habits on line bisection. , 1993, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[13]  P Bartolomeo,et al.  The relationship between visuospatial and representational neglect , 1994, Neurology.

[14]  D. Weinberger,et al.  Genetic variability of human brain size and cortical gyral patterns. , 1997, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[15]  S. Black,et al.  Brain-behavior correlations in hemispatial neglect using CT and SPECT , 1998, Neurology.

[16]  X Leclerc,et al.  Brain-behaviour relationships. Some models and related statistical procedures for the study of brain-damaged patients. , 1998, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[17]  J. Hurst,et al.  The botallo mystery , 1999 .

[18]  D. V. von Cramon,et al.  Sulcal variability of twins. , 1999, Cerebral cortex.

[19]  Mark E. McCourt,et al.  Visuospatial attention in line bisection: stimulusmodulation of pseudoneglect , 1999, Neuropsychologia.

[20]  Richard S. J. Frackowiak,et al.  Navigation-related structural change in the hippocampi of taxi drivers. , 2000, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[21]  M. McCourt,et al.  Pseudoneglect: a review and meta-analysis of performance factors in line bisection tasks , 2000, Neuropsychologia.

[22]  D. Schacter,et al.  Can medial temporal lobe regions distinguish true from false? An event-related functional MRI study of veridical and illusory recognition memory , 2001, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[23]  P. Bartolomeo L EVELS OF IMPAIRMENT IN UNILATERAL NEGLECT , 2001 .

[24]  Tatia M.C. Lee,et al.  Detection of malingering behavior at different levels of task difficulty in Hong kong Chinese , 2002 .

[25]  R. C. Gur,et al.  Brain Activity during Simulated Deception: An Event-Related Functional Magnetic Resonance Study , 2002, NeuroImage.

[26]  P. Garthwaite,et al.  Investigation of the single case in neuropsychology: confidence limits on the abnormality of test scores and test score differences , 2002, Neuropsychologia.

[27]  F. Tomaiuolo,et al.  The anatomy of neglect without hemianopia: a key role for parietal–frontal disconnection? , 2003, Neuroreport.

[28]  Bogdan Draganski,et al.  Neuroplasticity: Changes in grey matter induced by training , 2004, Nature.

[29]  Paul Root Wolpe,et al.  Monitoring and manipulating brain function: new neuroscience technologies and their ethical implications. , 2004, The Hastings Center report.

[30]  H. Duffau,et al.  Direct Evidence for a Parietal-Frontal Pathway Subserving Spatial Awareness in Humans , 2005, Science.

[31]  M. McCourt,et al.  Unilateral Visual Cueing and Asymmetric Line Geometry Share a Common Attentional Origin in the Modulation of Pseudoneglect , 2005, Cortex.

[32]  Paolo Bartolomeo,et al.  A parietofrontal network for spatial awareness in the right hemisphere of the human brain. , 2006, Archives of neurology.

[33]  M. Coltheart,et al.  Cognitive Neuropsychology twenty years on , 2006, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[34]  Derek K. Jones,et al.  Symmetries in human brain language pathways correlate with verbal recall , 2007, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[35]  Y. Rossetti,et al.  Looking while imagining , 2007, Neurology.

[36]  Marika Urbanski,et al.  Line bisection in left neglect: The importance of starting right , 2008, Cortex.

[37]  E. Vogel,et al.  CURRENT DIRECTIONS IN PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE How to Exploit Diversity for Scientific Gain Using Individual Differences to Constrain Cognitive Theory , 2022 .

[38]  P. Bartolomeo,et al.  Seeing and imagining the “same” objects in unilateral neglect , 2008, Neuropsychologia.

[39]  J. Lupiáñez,et al.  Left visual neglect: is the disengage deficit space- or object-based? , 2008, Experimental Brain Research.

[40]  Galia Avidan,et al.  Reduced structural connectivity in ventral visual cortex in congenital prosopagnosia , 2009, Nature Neuroscience.

[41]  C. Oppenheim,et al.  DTI-MR tractography of white matter damage in stroke patients with neglect , 2011, Experimental Brain Research.

[42]  S. Dehaene,et al.  How Learning to Read Changes the Cortical Networks for Vision and Language , 2010, Science.

[43]  J. Lupiáñez,et al.  Assessing the weights of visual neglect: A new approach to dissociate defective symptoms from productive phenomena in length estimation , 2010, Neuropsychologia.

[44]  T. Shallice,et al.  Right posterior cortical functions in a tumour patient series , 2010, Cortex.

[45]  K. Lovblad,et al.  Neuroanatomy of hemispatial neglect and its functional components: a study using voxel-based lesion-symptom mapping. , 2010, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[46]  Daniele Fanelli,et al.  Negative results are disappearing from most disciplines and countries , 2011, Scientometrics.

[47]  S. Dehaene,et al.  The unique role of the visual word form area in reading , 2011, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[48]  Russell A. Poldrack,et al.  Large-scale automated synthesis of human functional neuroimaging data , 2011, Nature Methods.

[49]  Patrick Cavanagh,et al.  Attention biases the perceived midpoint of horizontal lines , 2011, Neuropsychologia.

[50]  Paolo Bartolomeo,et al.  The quest for the ‘critical lesion site’ in cognitive deficits: Problems and perspectives , 2011, Cortex.

[51]  G. Rees,et al.  The structural basis of inter-individual differences in human behaviour and cognition , 2011, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[52]  P. Bartolomeo,et al.  Vocal response times to real and imagined stimuli in spatial neglect: A group study and single-case report , 2011, Cortex.

[53]  M. Catani,et al.  A lateralized brain network for visuospatial attention , 2011, Nature Neuroscience.

[54]  B. Dubois,et al.  Visuospatial deficits in posterior cortical atrophy: structural and functional correlates , 2012, Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry.

[55]  E. Yong Replication studies: Bad copy , 2012, Nature.

[56]  Massimo Filippi,et al.  Brain networks in posterior cortical atrophy: A single case tractography study and literature review , 2012, Cortex.

[57]  Jasiek Krzysztofiak IN THE WAKE OF HIGH-PROFILE CONTROVERSIES, PSYCHOLOGISTS ARE FACING UP TO PROBLEMS WITH REPLICATION. , 2012 .

[58]  J. Lupiáñez,et al.  Dissecting the component deficits of perceptual imbalance in visual neglect: Evidence from horizontal–vertical length comparisons , 2012, Cortex.

[59]  P. Bartolomeo,et al.  Visual neglect as a disconnection syndrome? A confirmatory case report , 2013, Neurocase.

[60]  Y Kamitani,et al.  Neural Decoding of Visual Imagery During Sleep , 2013, Science.

[61]  B. Dubois,et al.  Visuospatial deficits and hemispheric perfusion asymmetries in posterior cortical atrophy , 2013, Cortex.

[62]  G. Rees,et al.  Human brain lesion-deficit inference remapped , 2014, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[63]  Flavio Dell'Acqua,et al.  Anatomical predictors of aphasia recovery: a tractography study of bilateral perisylvian language networks. , 2014, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[64]  Stanislas Dehaene,et al.  Learning to read improves the structure of the arcuate fasciculus. , 2014, Cerebral cortex.

[65]  G. Tononi,et al.  Diaschisis: past, present, future. , 2014, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[66]  Massimo Silvetti,et al.  Damage to white matter pathways in subacute and chronic spatial neglect: a group study and 2 single-case studies with complete virtual "in vivo" tractography dissection. , 2012, Cerebral cortex.

[67]  P. König,et al.  Spatial biases in viewing behavior. , 2014, Journal of vision.

[68]  G. Vallar,et al.  Cerebral correlates of visuospatial neglect: A direct cerebral stimulation study , 2014, Human brain mapping.

[69]  P. Bartolomeo,et al.  White matter lesional predictors of chronic visual neglect: a longitudinal study. , 2015, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[70]  H. Duffau,et al.  Functional reorganization of the attentional networks in low-grade glioma patients: A longitudinal study , 2015, Cortex.

[71]  M. Chun,et al.  Functional connectome fingerprinting: Identifying individuals based on patterns of brain connectivity , 2015, Nature Neuroscience.

[72]  Régine Kolinsky,et al.  Illiterate to literate: behavioural and cerebral changes induced by reading acquisition , 2015, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[73]  Liang Wang,et al.  Probabilistic Maps of Visual Topography in Human Cortex. , 2015, Cerebral cortex.

[74]  R. Adolphs,et al.  Building a Science of Individual Differences from fMRI , 2016, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[75]  John P. A. Ioannidis,et al.  What does research reproducibility mean? , 2016, Science Translational Medicine.

[76]  P. Bartolomeo,et al.  Let thy left brain know what thy right brain doeth: Inter-hemispheric compensation of functional deficits after brain damage , 2016, Neuropsychologia.

[77]  Isabelle Peretz,et al.  Music and words in the visual cortex: The impact of musical expertise , 2017, Cortex.