Dyslexia and the magnocellular-parvocellular coactivaton hypothesis
暂无分享,去创建一个
Luca Battaglini | Clara Casco | Michele Barollo | Giulio Contemori | Ambra Ciavarelli | C. Casco | L. Battaglini | G. Contemori | Michele Barollo | Ambra Ciavarelli | Giulio Contemori | Luca Battaglini
[1] U. Polat,et al. The architecture of perceptual spatial interactions , 1994, Vision Research.
[2] M. Livingstone,et al. Physiological and anatomical evidence for a magnocellular defect in developmental dyslexia. , 1991, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[3] J. Stein,et al. Visual motion sensitivity in dyslexia: evidence for temporal and energy integration deficits , 2000, Neuropsychologia.
[4] T. Nealey,et al. Magnocellular and parvocellular contributions to responses in the middle temporal visual area (MT) of the macaque monkey , 1990, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.
[5] James M. G. Tsui,et al. Brief motion stimuli preferentially activate surround-suppressed neurons in macaque visual area MT , 2008, Current Biology.
[6] D H Brainard,et al. The Psychophysics Toolbox. , 1997, Spatial vision.
[7] D. Melcher,et al. Investigating the role of temporal processing in developmental dyslexia: Evidence for a specific deficit in rapid visual segmentation , 2020, Psychonomic Bulletin & Review.
[8] S. Leat,et al. Development of Visual Acuity and Contrast Sensitivity in Children , 2009 .
[9] Henry J. Alitto,et al. Function of inhibition in visual cortical processing , 2010, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.
[10] S. Gori,et al. How the visual aspects can be crucial in reading acquisition? The intriguing case of crowding and developmental dyslexia. , 2015, Journal of vision.
[11] Jan Churan,et al. Interaction of spatial and temporal factors in psychophysical estimates of surround suppression. , 2009, Journal of vision.
[12] J. Wouters,et al. Coherent motion sensitivity and reading development in the transition from prereading to reading stage. , 2011, Child development.
[13] A. Angelucci,et al. Contribution of feedforward, lateral and feedback connections to the classical receptive field center and extra-classical receptive field surround of primate V1 neurons. , 2006, Progress in brain research.
[14] T. Lawton. Improving Dorsal Stream Function in Dyslexics by Training Figure/Ground Motion Discrimination Improves Attention, Reading Fluency, and Working Memory , 2016, Front. Hum. Neurosci..
[15] J. M. Hupé,et al. Cortical feedback improves discrimination between figure and background by V1, V2 and V3 neurons , 1998, Nature.
[16] Marco Zorzi,et al. Nested incremental modeling in the development of computational theories: the CDP+ model of reading aloud. , 2007, Psychological review.
[17] M. Morgan,et al. Observers can voluntarily shift their psychometric functions without losing sensitivity , 2011, Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics.
[18] Gianluca Campana,et al. Reducing Crowding by Weakening Inhibitory Lateral Interactions in the Periphery with Perceptual Learning , 2011, PloS one.
[19] Keith D. White,et al. Contrast sensitivity during saccadic eye movements , 1978, Vision Research.
[20] H. Wimmer,et al. A dual-route perspective on eye movements of dyslexic readers , 2010, Cognition.
[21] C. Casco,et al. Developmental dyslexia: A deficit in magnocellular-parvocellular co-activation, not simply in pure magnocellular activation , 2019, Vision Research.
[22] P. Lennie,et al. Spatial and temporal contrast sensitivities of neurones in lateral geniculate nucleus of macaque. , 1984, The Journal of physiology.
[23] Lawrence C. Sincich,et al. The circuitry of V1 and V2: integration of color, form, and motion. , 2005, Annual review of neuroscience.
[24] R. Woods,et al. Abnormal processing of visual motion in dyslexia revealed by functional brain imaging , 1996, Nature.
[25] C. Distler,et al. Attention and normalization circuits in macaque V1 , 2015, The European journal of neuroscience.
[26] J. Stein,et al. The magnocellular theory of developmental dyslexia. , 2001, Dyslexia.
[27] B. Skottun,et al. Temporal Frequency and the Magnocellular and Parvocellular Systems , 2008 .
[28] Neil W. Roach,et al. Impaired filtering of behaviourally irrelevant visual information in dyslexia. , 2007, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[29] S. Gori,et al. A different vision of dyslexia: Local precedence on global perception , 2017, Scientific Reports.
[30] S. Gori,et al. Perceptual learning as a possible new approach for remediation and prevention of developmental dyslexia , 2014, Vision Research.
[31] Michele Rucci,et al. Contrast sensitivity reveals an oculomotor strategy for temporally encoding space , 2018, bioRxiv.
[32] Simone Gori,et al. Multiple Causal Links Between Magnocellular-Dorsal Pathway Deficit and Developmental Dyslexia. , 2016, Cerebral cortex.
[33] D G Pelli,et al. The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: transforming numbers into movies. , 1997, Spatial vision.
[34] B. Skottun,et al. The Possible Relationship Between Visual Deficits and Dyslexia , 1999, Journal of learning disabilities.
[35] M. Montague,et al. The Proposed Changes for DSM-5 for SLD and ADHD , 2013, Journal of learning disabilities.
[36] J. Stein. The current status of the magnocellular theory of developmental dyslexia , 2019, Neuropsychologia.
[37] David Burr,et al. Suppression of the magnocellular pathway during saccades , 1996, Behavioural Brain Research.
[38] B. Skottun,et al. The magnocellular deficit theory of dyslexia: the evidence from contrast sensitivity , 2000, Vision Research.
[39] E. Callaway,et al. Parallel processing strategies of the primate visual system , 2009, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.
[40] J. Shelley-Tremblay,et al. Training on Movement Figure-Ground Discrimination Remediates Low-Level Visual Timing Deficits in the Dorsal Stream, Improving High-Level Cognitive Functioning, Including Attention, Reading Fluency, and Working Memory , 2017, Front. Hum. Neurosci..
[41] C. Casco,et al. Perceptual learning leads to long lasting visual improvement in patients with central vision loss. , 2016, Restorative neurology and neuroscience.
[42] Nicola J. Pitchford,et al. Encoding of rapid time-varying information is impaired in poor readers , 2017, Journal of vision.
[43] P. Goolkasian,et al. Letter identification and lateral masking in dyslexic and average readers. , 1990, The American journal of psychology.
[44] Merav Ahissar,et al. Disabled readers suffer from visual and auditory impairments but not from a specific magnocellular deficit. , 2002, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[45] B Suresh Krishna,et al. Surround Suppression Sharpens the Priority Map in the Lateral Intraparietal Area , 2022 .
[46] S. Crewther,et al. Towards an understanding of the role of the ‘magnocellular advantage’ in fluent reading , 2008, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.
[47] Ignacio Serrano-Pedraza,et al. Two Common Psychophysical Measures of Surround Suppression Reflect Independent Neuronal Mechanisms Task 1: Motion-direction Discrimination Task 2: Contrast Detection Suppression Index (si) , 2022 .
[48] M Coltheart,et al. DRC: a dual route cascaded model of visual word recognition and reading aloud. , 2001, Psychological review.
[49] D. Heeger,et al. The Normalization Model of Attention , 2009, Neuron.
[50] D. Giaschi,et al. M-stream deficits and reading-related visual processes in developmental dyslexia. , 2007, Psychological bulletin.
[51] C. Casco,et al. Perceptual learning improves contrast sensitivity, visual acuity, and foveal crowding in amblyopia. , 2017, Restorative neurology and neuroscience.
[52] Duje Tadin,et al. Optimal size for perceiving motion decreases with contrast , 2005, Vision Research.
[53] S. Gori,et al. Is excessive visual crowding causally linked to developmental dyslexia? , 2019, Neuropsychologia.
[54] D. Spinelli,et al. Crowding, reading, and developmental dyslexia. , 2009, Journal of vision.
[55] Marco Zorzi,et al. The Development of Spelling-Sound Relationships in a Model of Phonological Reading , 1998 .
[56] Duje Tadin,et al. A Substantial and Unexpected Enhancement of Motion Perception in Autism , 2013, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[57] H. Levitt. Transformed up-down methods in psychoacoustics. , 1971, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.
[58] J. Krystal,et al. Impairment of GABAergic transmission in depression: new insights from neuroimaging studies. , 2000, Critical reviews in neurobiology.
[59] E. Callaway,et al. Convergence of magno- and parvocellular pathways in layer 4B of macaque primary visual cortex , 1996, Nature.
[60] A. Thiele,et al. Comparison of spatial integration and surround suppression characteristics in spiking activity and the local field potential in macaque V1 , 2008, The European journal of neuroscience.
[61] Jong H. Yoon,et al. GABA Concentration Is Reduced in Visual Cortex in Schizophrenia and Correlates with Orientation-Specific Surround Suppression , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[62] M. Zorzi,et al. Two routes or one in reading aloud? A connectionist dual-process model. , 1998 .
[63] James L. McClelland,et al. Understanding normal and impaired word reading: computational principles in quasi-regular domains. , 1996, Psychological review.
[64] Randolph Blake,et al. Perceptual consequences of centre–surround antagonism in visual motion processing , 2003, Nature.
[65] Nicholas Shenker,et al. Neurocognitive and Neuroplastic Mechanisms of Novel Clinical Signs in CRPS , 2016, Front. Hum. Neurosci..
[66] Nicola J. Pitchford,et al. Visual perception in dyslexia is limited by sub-optimal scale selection , 2017, Scientific Reports.
[67] C. Casco,et al. Suppressive effects on motion discrimination induced by transient flankers are reduced by perceptual learning. , 2015, Journal of vision.
[68] D. Heeger,et al. Brain activity in visual cortex predicts individual differences in reading performance. , 1997, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[69] A. Angelucci,et al. Multiple components of surround modulation in primary visual cortex: Multiple neural circuits with multiple functions? , 2014, Vision Research.
[70] C. Miniussi,et al. Excitatory and inhibitory lateral interactions effects on contrast detection are modulated by tRNS , 2019, Scientific Reports.
[71] M. Sigman,et al. Opinion TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences Vol.9 No.7 July 2005 The neural code for written words: a proposal , 2022 .
[72] Duje Tadin,et al. Mechanisms of motion-based object segregation , 2014 .
[73] L. P. O'Keefe,et al. The influence of fixational eye movements on the response of neurons in area MT of the macaque , 1998, Visual Neuroscience.
[74] T. Whitlock. Dyslexia , 1989, The Lancet.
[75] B. Skottun. On the use of spatial frequency to isolate contributions from the magnocellular and parvocellular systems and the dorsal and ventral cortical streams , 2015, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.