A New Look at Musical Expectancy: The Veridical Versus the General in the Mental Organization of Music

This paper takes a step back from what we label ‘problem solving’ approaches to the psychology of music memory and processing. In contrast with generalised expectation theories of music processing, a hypothesis is proposed which is based on the idea that mental representations of music are largely based on a large library of individual pieces: Case-based memory. We argue that it is the activation of these memories that forms the critical aspect of musical experience. Furthermore, a specific hypothesis is proposed that it is possible to represent any new piece of music through the chaining together of different, pre-existing veridical segments of music, in contrast with ‘problem solving by generalization’ which determines expectation based on statistical/stylistic/schematic factors. By adjusting segment length, or by forming new segments through repeated listenings, new music can be absorbed into an existing, growing mental database by chaining together existing veridical segments that match the incoming stimulus.

[1]  Adam N. Sanborn,et al.  Unifying rational models of categorization via the hierarchical Dirichlet process , 2019 .

[2]  Adrian C. North,et al.  A dynamically minimalist cognitive explanation of musical preference: is familiarity everything? , 2014, Front. Psychol..

[3]  R. Zajonc Attitudinal effects of mere exposure. , 1968 .

[4]  Marcus T. Pearce,et al.  The construction and evaluation of statistical models of melodic structure in music perception and composition , 2005 .

[5]  J. Bharucha,et al.  Modularity in musical processing: the automaticity of harmonic priming. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[6]  B. Tillmann,et al.  Musical structure processing after repeated listening: Schematic expectations resist veridical expectations , 2010 .

[7]  Harold L. Somers 3. Translation memory systems , 2003 .

[8]  Colin Martindale,et al.  Priming, prototypicality, and preference. , 1988 .

[9]  M. Pearce,et al.  Sweet Anticipation : Music and the Psychology of Expectation , 2007 .

[10]  J. Bharucha Tonality and expectation. , 1994 .

[11]  Janet L. Kolodner,et al.  Case-Based Reasoning , 1988, IJCAI 1989.

[12]  Geraint A. Wiggins,et al.  Improved Methods for Statistical Modelling of Monophonic Music , 2004 .

[13]  Christopher K. Riesbeck,et al.  Inside Case-Based Reasoning , 1989 .

[14]  Leif Finnas,et al.  How Can Musical Preferences Be Modified? A Research Review. , 1989 .

[15]  Suzanne Bunton,et al.  Semantically Motivated Improvements for PPM Variants , 1997, Comput. J..

[16]  David Huron,et al.  The first three seconds , 2011 .

[17]  R. Zajonc Mere Exposure: A Gateway to the Subliminal , 2001 .

[18]  Janet L. Kolodner,et al.  Case-Based Reasoning , 1989, IJCAI 1989.

[19]  J. Bharucha Music Cognition and Perceptual Facilitation: A Connectionist Framework , 1987 .

[20]  C. Krumhansl Music as Cognition. , 1987 .

[21]  Kristian J. Hammond,et al.  A Cognitive Science Approach to Case-Based Planning , 1993 .

[22]  J. Bharucha,et al.  Varieties of musical experience , 2006, Cognition.

[23]  Allan Collins,et al.  A spreading-activation theory of semantic processing , 1975 .

[24]  Emery Schubert,et al.  The fundamental function of music , 2009 .

[25]  J. Bharucha,et al.  Priming of chords: Spreading activation or overlapping frequency spectra? , 1987, Perception & psychophysics.

[26]  Adrian C. North,et al.  Imagination and creativity in music listening , 2011 .

[27]  Emery Schubert,et al.  Loved music can make a listener feel negative emotions , 2013 .

[28]  Geraint A. Wiggins,et al.  EXPECTATION IN MELODY: THE INFLUENCE OF CONTEXT AND LEARNING , 2006 .

[29]  Eric Heinze,et al.  Concepts and Contexts , 2016, The Russian Graphosphere, 1450-1850.

[30]  Göran Folkestad,et al.  Digital tools and discourse in music: The ecology of composition , 2011 .

[31]  Emery Schubert Enjoyment of Negative Emotions in Music: An Associative Network Explanation , 1996 .

[32]  David Temperley,et al.  A Probabilistic Model of Melody Perception , 2008, ISMIR.

[33]  Stephen Davies,et al.  Musical Meaning and Expression , 1994 .

[34]  Roger C. Schank,et al.  Scripts, plans, goals and understanding: an inquiry into human knowledge structures , 1978 .

[35]  David J. Hargreaves,et al.  Musical imagination: Perception and production, beauty and creativity , 2012 .

[36]  Robert O. Gjerdingen,et al.  Scanning the Dial: The Rapid Recognition of Music Genres , 2008 .

[37]  D. Temperley The Cognition of Basic Musical Structures , 2001 .

[38]  M. Crochemore,et al.  On-line construction of suffix trees , 2002 .

[39]  S. Tomkins,et al.  Script theory: differential magnification of affects. , 1978, Nebraska Symposium on Motivation. Nebraska Symposium on Motivation.

[40]  Paul Iverson,et al.  Name that tune: Identifying popular recordings from brief excerpts , 1999, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[41]  Eliot R. Smith,et al.  Exemplar and Prototype Use in Social Categorization , 1990 .

[42]  Leonard B. Meyer Emotion and Meaning in Music , 1957 .

[43]  Peter M. Todd,et al.  Modeling the Perception of Tonal Structure with Neural Nets , 1989 .

[44]  E. Narmour The Top-down and Bottom-up Systems of Musical Implication: Building on Meyer's Theory of Emotional Syntax , 1991 .

[45]  James Stewart,et al.  Calculus: Concepts and Contexts , 1999 .

[46]  E. Oja Simplified neuron model as a principal component analyzer , 1982, Journal of mathematical biology.

[47]  Colin Martindale,et al.  The pleasures of thought: A theory of cognitive hedonics. , 1984 .

[48]  Eugene Narmour,et al.  The Analysis and Cognition of Basic Melodic Structures: The Implication-Realization Model , 1990 .

[49]  John G. Cleary,et al.  Unbounded Length Contexts for PPM , 1997 .

[50]  Geraint A. Wiggins,et al.  Chapter 1 Computational Modelling of Music Cognition and Musical Creativity , 2008 .