Using Robust Normative Data to Investigate the Neuropsychology of Cognitive Aging

Objective The extent to which increasing age is associated with impairment in cognitive function, termed cognitive aging, may have been overestimated in prior studies. The inclusion of individuals with severe or uncontrolled systemic medical illness or prodromal neurodegenerative disease in normal aging samples is likely to bias estimates toward lower cognitive performance and inflate estimates of variability. Method Unbiased estimates of cognitive aging in 658 adults aged 60-84, who underwent rigorous screening to ensure their general and cognitive health, were computed. The first study screened the psychometric properties of a battery of neuropsychological tests in order to identify those with optimal properties to evaluate cognitive aging. The second study used the selected tests to compare baseline performance within 5-year age bands from 60 to 84. Results The first study identified a battery of 12 tests that provided reliable measures of memory, psychomotor speed, attention, and executive function and were appropriate for investigating age-related cognitive changes. The second study observed moderate to large age-related impairment for performance on tests of complex psychomotor function, category fluency, verbal learning, and verbal and visual memory. No, or only small, age effects were observed for working memory, phonemic fluency, learning of visual information, and reaction time. Conclusions These data suggested that while increasing age is associated with impairment in cognitive function, this impairment is less severe and is evident only on more complex neuropsychological tests than estimated previously in samples selected using less rigorous criteria to ensure cognitive health.

[1]  Sterling C. Johnson,et al.  Mild Cognitive Impairment in Late Middle Age in the Wisconsin Registry for Alzheimer's Prevention Study: Prevalence and Characteristics Using Robust and Standard Neuropsychological Normative Data. , 2016, Archives of clinical neuropsychology : the official journal of the National Academy of Neuropsychologists.

[2]  D. Delis,et al.  The California verbal learning test , 2016 .

[3]  Terry K Koo,et al.  A Guideline of Selecting and Reporting Intraclass Correlation Coefficients for Reliability Research. , 2016, Journal of chiropractic medicine.

[4]  C. Browning,et al.  Dual Sensory Loss in Older Adults: A Systematic Review. , 2015, The Gerontologist.

[5]  Wenzhu Mowrey,et al.  Conventional and robust norming in identifying preclinical dementia , 2015, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[6]  R. Hertwig,et al.  The role of cognitive abilities in decisions from experience: Age differences emerge as a function of choice set size , 2015, Cognition.

[7]  S. Clouston,et al.  Educational inequalities in aging-related declines in fluid cognition and the onset of cognitive pathology , 2015, Alzheimer's & dementia.

[8]  D. Aarsland,et al.  Cognitive performance and neuropsychiatric symptoms in early, untreated Parkinson's disease , 2015, Movement disorders : official journal of the Movement Disorder Society.

[9]  Scott W. Brown,et al.  Executive attentional resources in timing: Effects of inhibitory control and cognitive aging. , 2015, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[10]  Thomas Dohmen,et al.  Risk attitude and cognitive aging , 2015 .

[11]  M. Albert,et al.  Diabetes in Midlife and Cognitive Change Over 20 Years , 2014, Annals of Internal Medicine.

[12]  T. Wolbers,et al.  How cognitive aging affects multisensory integration of navigational cues , 2014, Neurobiology of Aging.

[13]  U. Lindenberger Human cognitive aging: Corriger la fortune? , 2014, Science.

[14]  Karen Bandeen-Roche,et al.  Midlife hypertension and 20-year cognitive change: the atherosclerosis risk in communities neurocognitive study. , 2014, JAMA neurology.

[15]  L. Fratiglioni,et al.  Incidence and Predictors of Multimorbidity in the Elderly: A Population-Based Longitudinal Study , 2014, PloS one.

[16]  Taina Rantanen,et al.  Mobility Decline in Old Age , 2013, Exercise and sport sciences reviews.

[17]  Meghan B. Mitchell,et al.  Social Activity and Cognitive Functioning Over Time: A Coordinated Analysis of Four Longitudinal Studies , 2012, Journal of aging research.

[18]  C. Rowe,et al.  Use of the CogState Brief Battery in the assessment of Alzheimer's disease related cognitive impairment in the Australian Imaging, Biomarkers and Lifestyle (AIBL) study , 2012, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[19]  K. Schaie,et al.  Cohort differences in cognitive aging and terminal decline in the Seattle Longitudinal Study. , 2011, Developmental psychology.

[20]  D. Bowers,et al.  The Case for Testing Memory With Both Stories and Word Lists Prior to DBS Surgery for Parkinson's Disease , 2011, The Clinical neuropsychologist.

[21]  J. Morris,et al.  Ascertainment bias in the clinical diagnosis of Alzheimer disease. , 2010, Archives of neurology.

[22]  T. Salthouse Selective review of cognitive aging , 2010, Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society.

[23]  R. Petersen,et al.  Robust and expanded norms for the Dementia Rating Scale. , 2010, Archives of clinical neuropsychology : the official journal of the National Academy of Neuropsychologists.

[24]  M. Eckert,et al.  Age-related differences in gap detection: Effects of task difficulty and cognitive ability , 2010, Hearing Research.

[25]  C. Jack,et al.  Alzheimer's Disease Neuroimaging Initiative (ADNI) , 2010, Neurology.

[26]  R. Marioni,et al.  Age-associated cognitive decline. , 2009, British medical bulletin.

[27]  C. Rowe,et al.  The Australian Imaging, Biomarkers and Lifestyle (AIBL) study of aging: methodology and baseline characteristics of 1112 individuals recruited for a longitudinal study of Alzheimer's disease , 2009, International Psychogeriatrics.

[28]  A. Saykin,et al.  Differential Memory Test Sensitivity for Diagnosing Amnestic Mild Cognitive Impairment and Predicting Conversion to Alzheimer's Disease , 2009, Neuropsychology, development, and cognition. Section B, Aging, neuropsychology and cognition.

[29]  I. Deary,et al.  Smarter in middle age, faster in old age: a cross-lagged panel analysis of reaction time and cognitive ability over 13 years in the West of Scotland Twenty-07 Study. , 2009, Psychology and aging.

[30]  Herman Buschke,et al.  Robust norms for selected neuropsychological tests in older adults. , 2008, Archives of clinical neuropsychology : the official journal of the National Academy of Neuropsychologists.

[31]  B. Reisberg,et al.  Robust and conventional neuropsychological norms: diagnosis and prediction of age-related cognitive decline. , 2008, Neuropsychology.

[32]  V. Pankratz,et al.  The Mayo Clinic Study of Aging: Design and Sampling, Participation, Baseline Measures and Sample Characteristics , 2008, Neuroepidemiology.

[33]  R. Green,et al.  Geriatric Performance on an Abbreviated Version of the Boston Naming Test , 2007, Applied neuropsychology.

[34]  O. Almkvist,et al.  The importance of impaired physical health and age in normal cognitive aging. , 2007, Scandinavian journal of psychology.

[35]  J. Jolles,et al.  Cognitive Functioning in Healthy Older Adults Aged 64–81: A Cohort Study into the Effects of Age, Sex, and Education , 2007, Neuropsychology, development, and cognition. Section B, Aging, neuropsychology and cognition.

[36]  K Warner Schaie,et al.  What Can We Learn From Longitudinal Studies of Adult Development? , 2005, Research in human development.

[37]  C. Jack,et al.  Mild cognitive impairment – beyond controversies, towards a consensus: report of the International Working Group on Mild Cognitive Impairment , 2004, Journal of internal medicine.

[38]  G Ratcliff,et al.  Normative Data on the Boston Naming Test and Two Equivalent 30-Item Short Forms , 2000, The Clinical neuropsychologist.

[39]  B. Agrell,et al.  The clock-drawing test. 1998. , 1998, Age and ageing.

[40]  C. Caltagirone,et al.  Memory performances in young, elderly, and very old healthy individuals versus patients with Alzheimer's disease: evidence for discontinuity between normal and pathological aging. , 1998, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[41]  H. Buschke,et al.  The effects of preclinical dementia on estimates of normal cognitive functioning in aging. , 1996, The journals of gerontology. Series B, Psychological sciences and social sciences.

[42]  H. Levin A Compendium of Neuropsychological Tests: Administration, Norms, and Commentary , 1993 .

[43]  M. Folstein,et al.  Clinical diagnosis of Alzheimer's disease , 1984, Neurology.

[44]  A. Zigmond,et al.  The Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale , 1983, Acta psychiatrica Scandinavica.

[45]  T. Salthouse In press, Current Directions in Psychological Science Why are there different age relations in cross-sectional and longitudinal comparisons of cognitive functioning? , 2014 .

[46]  P. Vliet Cholesterol and Late-Life Cognitive Decline , 2012 .

[47]  Ian J Deary,et al.  Are processing speed tasks biomarkers of cognitive aging? , 2010, Psychology and aging.

[48]  J. J. Ryan,et al.  Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-III , 2001 .

[49]  C. Reynolds,et al.  Wechsler memory scale-revised , 1988 .