Facilitating sensorimotor integration via blocked practice underpins imitation learning of atypical biological kinematics in autism spectrum disorder
暂无分享,去创建一个
D. Elliott | S. Bennett | G. Bird | J. Causer | S. Hayes | N. Foster | Nathan C. Foster | Joe Causer
[1] R. Greene,et al. Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule (ADOS-2) elevations in a clinical sample of children and adolescents who do not have autism: Phenotypic profiles of false positives , 2021, The Clinical neuropsychologist.
[2] G. Vivanti,et al. Object-directed imitation in autism spectrum disorder is differentially influenced by motoric task complexity, but not social contextual cues , 2019, Autism : the international journal of research and practice.
[3] J. Parr,et al. Making the future together: Shaping autism research through meaningful participation , 2018, Autism : the international journal of research and practice.
[4] I. Koch,et al. Intact mirror mechanisms for automatic facial emotions in children and adolescents with autism spectrum disorder , 2017, Autism research : official journal of the International Society for Autism Research.
[5] Mark H. Johnson,et al. Interaction takes two: Typical adults exhibit mind-blindness towards those with autism spectrum disorder. , 2016, Journal of abnormal psychology.
[6] W. Verwey,et al. Consolidating behavioral and neurophysiologic findings to explain the influence of contextual interference during motor sequence learning , 2016, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[7] D. Elliott,et al. Complimentary lower-level and higher-order systems underpin imitation learning , 2016, Brain and Cognition.
[8] Digby Elliott,et al. Low Fidelity Imitation of Atypical Biological Kinematics in Autism Spectrum Disorders Is Modulated by Self-Generated Selective Attention , 2015, Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders.
[9] Geoffrey Bird,et al. Intact Automatic Imitation and Typical Spatial Compatibility in Autism Spectrum Disorder: Challenging the Broken Mirror Theory , 2015, Autism research : official journal of the International Society for Autism Research.
[10] Digby Elliott,et al. Top-down attentional processes modulate the coding of atypical biological motion kinematics in the absence of motor signals. , 2014, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[11] A. Hamilton,et al. Imitation in Autism Spectrum Disorders , 2014 .
[12] John O. Willis,et al. Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence , 2014 .
[13] R. McIntosh,et al. A Specific Deficit of Imitation in Autism Spectrum Disorder , 2013, Autism research : official journal of the International Society for Autism Research.
[14] P. Brambilla,et al. Preserved Imitation of Known Gestures in Children with High-Functioning Autism , 2013, ISRN neurology.
[15] D. Elliott,et al. Visual online control processes are acquired during observational practice. , 2013, Acta psychologica.
[16] A. Hamilton,et al. Reflecting on the mirror neuron system in autism: A systematic review of current theories , 2013, Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience.
[17] S. Swinnen,et al. Recognizing Biological Motion and Emotions from Point-Light Displays in Autism Spectrum Disorders , 2012, PloS one.
[18] E. Poliakoff,et al. Goal-Directed and Goal-Less Imitation in Autism Spectrum Disorder , 2011, Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders.
[19] C. Winstein,et al. Learning–performance distinction and memory processes for motor skills: A focused review and perspective , 2012, Behavioural Brain Research.
[20] Simon J. Bennett,et al. Dissociable contributions of motor-execution and action-observation to intramanual transfer , 2012, Experimental Brain Research.
[21] D. Wolpert,et al. Principles of sensorimotor learning , 2011, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.
[22] Dora M Raymaker,et al. Collaboration Strategies in Nontraditional Community-Based Participatory Research Partnerships: Lessons From an Academic–Community Partnership With Autistic Self-Advocates , 2011, Progress in community health partnerships : research, education, and action.
[23] M. Brass,et al. Hyperimitation of Actions Is Related to Reduced Understanding of Others' Minds in Autism Spectrum Conditions , 2010, Biological Psychiatry.
[24] James L. Lyons,et al. Goal-directed aiming: two components but multiple processes. , 2010, Psychological bulletin.
[25] Ayse Pinar Saygin,et al. Unaffected Perceptual Thresholds for Biological and Non-Biological Form-from-Motion Perception in Autism Spectrum Conditions , 2010, PloS one.
[26] Daniel C. Richardson,et al. Intact imitation of emotional facial actions in autism spectrum conditions , 2010, Neuropsychologia.
[27] Simon J. Bennett,et al. General motor representations are developed during action-observation , 2010, Experimental Brain Research.
[28] Susan S. Jones,et al. The development of imitation in infancy , 2009, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[29] Richard J. Krauzlis,et al. Spatial allocation of attention during smooth pursuit eye movements , 2009, Vision Research.
[30] C. Heyes,et al. Weak imitative performance is not due to a functional ‘mirroring’ deficit in adults with Autism Spectrum Disorders , 2008, Neuropsychologia.
[31] A. Hamilton,et al. Imitation and action understanding in autistic spectrum disorders: How valid is the hypothesis of a deficit in the mirror neuron system? , 2007, Neuropsychologia.
[32] C. Heyes,et al. Intact automatic imitation of human and robot actions in autism spectrum disorders , 2007, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[33] G. Dawson,et al. EEG mu rhythm and imitation impairments in individuals with autism spectrum disorder , 2007, Brain and Cognition.
[34] James L. Lyons,et al. A kinematic analysis of how young adults with and without autism plan and control goal-directed movements. , 2006, Motor control.
[35] Gereon R. Fink,et al. Common and Differential Neural Mechanisms Supporting Imitation of Meaningful and Meaningless Actions , 2005, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[36] M. Guadagnoli,et al. Challenge Point: A Framework for Conceptualizing the Effects of Various Practice Conditions in Motor Learning , 2004, Journal of motor behavior.
[37] A. Whiten,et al. A Systematic Review of Action Imitation in Autistic Spectrum Disorder , 2004, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.
[38] G. Barnes,et al. Human ocular pursuit during the transient disappearance of a visual target. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.
[39] D. Perrett,et al. Imitation, mirror neurons and autism , 2001, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.
[40] W. Helsen,et al. A century later: Woodworth's (1899) two-component model of goal-directed aiming. , 2001, Psychological bulletin.
[41] D. Wright,et al. Motor programming during practice conditions high and low in contextual interference. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[42] N. Rinehart,et al. Movement Preparation in High-Functioning Autism and Asperger Disorder: A Serial Choice Reaction Time Task Involving Motor Reprogramming , 2001, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.
[43] D. Wright,et al. An Assessment of the Attention Demands during Random- and Blocked-Practice Schedules , 2000, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.
[44] R. Hobson,et al. Imitation and identification in autism. , 1999, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.
[45] F. Brady. A Theoretical and Empirical Review of the Contextual Interference Effect and the Learning of Motor Skills , 1998 .
[46] W. Prinz. Perception and Action Planning , 1997 .
[47] B. Pennington,et al. Imitation and pantomime in high-functioning adolescents with autism spectrum disorders. , 1996, Child development.
[48] B. Pennington,et al. A theoretical approach to the deficits in infantile autism , 1991, Development and Psychopathology.
[49] R. Magill,et al. A REVIEW OF THE CONTEXTUAL INTERFERENCE EFFECT IN MOTOR SKILL ACQUISITION , 1990 .
[50] Bruce Thompson,et al. Planned versus Unplanned and Orthogonal versus Nonorthogonal Contrasts: The Neo-Classical Perspective. , 1990 .
[51] D. Elliott,et al. Contextual Interference Effects during Skill Acquisition and Transfer in Down’s Syndrome Adolescents , 1986 .
[52] T. Flash,et al. The coordination of arm movements: an experimentally confirmed mathematical model , 1985, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.
[53] J. Shea,et al. Contextual interference effects on the acquisition, retention, and transfer of a motor skill. , 1979 .
[54] W. Demyer,et al. Imitation in autistic, early schizophrenic, and non-psychotic subnormal children , 1972, Journal of autism and childhood schizophrenia.
[55] Digby Elliott,et al. Atypical biological motion kinematics are represented by complementary lower-level and top-down processes during imitation learning. , 2016, Acta psychologica.
[56] C. Heyes,et al. Imitation in infancy: the wealth of the stimulus. , 2011, Developmental science.
[57] P. van Donkelaar,et al. The allocation of attention during smooth pursuit eye movements. , 2002, Progress in brain research.
[58] W. Prinz,et al. Movement observation affects movement execution in a simple response task. , 2001, Acta psychologica.