Interference in the shared-Stroop task: a comparison of self- and other-monitoring
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] Joris Van de Cavey,et al. Interference in joint picture naming. , 2015, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[2] J. Bavelas,et al. Listeners as co-narrators. , 2000, Journal of personality and social psychology.
[3] Bernard Guerin,et al. Mere presence effects in humans: A review , 1986 .
[4] Colin M. Macleod. Half a century of research on the Stroop effect: an integrative review. , 1991, Psychological bulletin.
[5] M. Pickering,et al. Early preparation during turn-taking: Listeners use content predictions to determine what to say but not when to say it , 2018, Cognition.
[6] Eva Belke,et al. Language production in a shared task: Cumulative Semantic Interference from self- and other-produced context words. , 2016, Acta psychologica.
[8] Wolfgang Prinz,et al. What is Shared in Joint Action? Issues of Co-representation, Response Conflict, and Agent Identification , 2011 .
[9] Geert J. M. van Boxtel,et al. The N2 in go/no-go tasks reflects conflict monitoring not response inhibition , 2004, Brain and Cognition.
[10] Wen-Jui Kuo,et al. Action Co-representation is Tuned to Other Humans , 2008, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[11] W. Glaser,et al. Context effects in stroop-like word and picture processing. , 1989, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[12] Martin J. Pickering,et al. Edinburgh Research Explorer A cognitive architecture for the coordination of utterances , 2022 .
[13] G. Dell,et al. Is comprehension necessary for error detection? A conflict-based account of monitoring in speech production , 2011, Cognitive Psychology.
[14] Elena Rusconi,et al. Sharing a task or sharing space? On the effect of the confederate in action coding in a detection task , 2010, Cognition.
[15] Bernhard Hommel,et al. How Social Are Task Representations? , 2009, Psychological science.
[16] M. Pickering,et al. Predicting While Comprehending Language: A Theory and Review , 2018, Psychological bulletin.
[17] P. Jolicoeur,et al. A Solution to the Effect of Sample Size on Outlier Elimination , 1994 .
[18] M. Pickering,et al. Understanding Dialogue , 2021 .
[19] H. Bekkering,et al. Joint action: bodies and minds moving together , 2006, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[20] Wolfgang Prinz,et al. How “Social” is the social Simon effect? , 2010, Front. Psychology.
[21] Marcel Brass,et al. Does the Human Motor System Simulate Pinocchio's Actions? , 2007, Psychological science.
[22] M. Pickering,et al. How do speakers coordinate? Evidence for prediction in a joint word-replacement task , 2015, Cortex.
[23] Mante S. Nieuwland,et al. Neural correlates of verbal joint action: ERPs reveal common perception and action systems in a shared-Stroop task , 2016, Brain Research.
[24] P. Kay,et al. Universals and cultural variation in turn-taking in conversation , 2009, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[25] D. Melcher,et al. No evidence of task co-representation in a joint Stroop task , 2017, Psychological Research.
[26] Rasha Abdel Rahman,et al. Having a task partner affects lexical retrieval: Spoken word production in shared task settings , 2017, Cognition.
[27] Wolfgang Prinz,et al. The Virtual Co-Actor: The Social Simon Effect does not Rely on Online Feedback from the Other , 2010, Front. Psychology.
[28] Albert Costa,et al. On predicting others’ words: Electrophysiological evidence of prediction in speech production , 2014, Cognition.
[29] A. Voss,et al. Social Presence Effects on the Stroop Task: Boundary Conditions and an Alternative Account , 2008 .
[30] Henk Aarts,et al. When competition merges people's behavior: Interdependency activates shared action representations , 2010 .
[31] Jan-Peter de Holger N. J. Ruiter,et al. Projecting the End of a Speaker's Turn: A Cognitive Cornerstone of Conversation , 2006 .
[32] Matthew Ray,et al. Seeing vs. believing: Is believing sufficient to activate the processes of response co-representation? , 2007, Human movement science.
[33] J. Wicherts,et al. Heterogeneity in direct replications in psychology and its association with effect size. , 2020, Psychological bulletin.
[34] L. Ferrand,et al. The influence of mere social presence on Stroop interference: New evidence from the semantically-based Stroop task , 2012 .
[35] W. Levelt,et al. Monitoring and self-repair in speech , 1983, Cognition.
[36] Robert J. Hartsuiker,et al. Towards a New Model of Verbal Monitoring , 2020, Journal of cognition.
[37] Martin J. Pickering,et al. Human Neuroscience Hypothesis and Theory Article Self-, Other-, and Joint Monitoring Using Forward Models , 2022 .
[38] Laurel Brehm,et al. Mental representations of partner task cause interference in picture naming. , 2019, Acta psychologica.
[39] R. Cubelli,et al. When task sharing reduces interference: evidence for division-of-labour in Stroop-like tasks , 2018, Psychological Research.
[40] Wolfgang Prinz,et al. The (not so) social Simon effect: a referential coding account. , 2013, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.