Ideal cue combination for localizing texture-defined edges.

Many visual tasks can be carried out by using several sources of information. The most accurate estimates of scene properties require the observer to utilize all available information and to combine the information sources in an optimal manner. Two experiments are described that required the observers to judge the relative locations of two texture-defined edges (a vernier task). The edges were signaled by a change across the edge of two texture properties [either frequency and orientation (Experiment 1) or contrast and orientation (Experiment 2)]. The reliability of each cue was controlled by varying the distance over which the change (in frequency, orientation, or contrast) occurred-a kind of "texture blur." In some conditions, the position of the edge signaled by one cue was shifted relative to the other ("perturbation analysis"). An ideal-observer model, previously used in studies of depth perception and color constancy, was fitted to the data. Although the fit can be rejected relative to some more elaborate models, especially given the large quantity of data, this model does account for most trends in the data. A second, suboptimal model that switches between the available cues from trial to trial does a poor job of accounting for the data.

[1]  J. Wolfowitz,et al.  An Introduction to the Theory of Statistics , 1951, Nature.

[2]  M. Kendall,et al.  The advanced theory of statistics , 1945 .

[3]  Franklin A. Graybill,et al.  Introduction to The theory , 1974 .

[4]  H. Akaike A new look at the statistical model identification , 1974 .

[5]  Lloyd Kaufman,et al.  Sight and mind , 1974 .

[6]  R. M. Boynton,et al.  Visibility of borders: separate and combined effects of color differences, luminance contrast, and luminance level. , 1981, Journal of the Optical Society of America.

[7]  Michael S. Landy,et al.  HIPS: A unix-based image processing system , 1984, Comput. Vis. Graph. Image Process..

[8]  A. Derrington,et al.  Separate detectors for simple and complex grating patterns? , 1985, Vision Research.

[9]  G. Westheimer,et al.  Spatial location and hyperacuity: The centre/surround localization contribution function has two substrates , 1985, Vision Research.

[10]  D. Massaro Ambiguity in perception and experimentation. , 1988, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[11]  Michael S. Landy,et al.  A Statistical Framework for Robust Fusion of Depth Information , 1989, Other Conferences.

[12]  John Y. Aloimonos,et al.  Unification and integration of visual modules: an extension of the Marr Paradigm , 1989 .

[13]  D. Massaro,et al.  Models of integration given multiple sources of information. , 1990, Psychological review.

[14]  James J. Clark,et al.  Data Fusion for Sensory Information Processing Systems , 1990 .

[15]  J. Bergen,et al.  Computational Modeling of Visual Texture Segregation , 1991 .

[16]  J. Bergen,et al.  Texture segregation and orientation gradient , 1991, Vision Research.

[17]  J. Cutting,et al.  Selectivity, scope, and simplicity of models: a lesson from fitting judgments of perceived depth. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[18]  M. Landy,et al.  A perturbation analysis of depth perception from combinations of texture and motion cues , 1993, Vision Research.

[19]  Michael S. Landy,et al.  Combining multiple cues for texture edge localization , 1993, Electronic Imaging.

[20]  D. Massaro,et al.  The paradigm and the fuzzy logical model of perception are alive and well. , 1993, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[21]  Andrew T. Smith,et al.  Evidence for separate motion-detecting mechanisms for first- and second-order motion in human vision , 1994, Vision Research.

[22]  H R Wilson,et al.  A model for motion coherence and transparency , 1994, Visual Neuroscience.

[23]  Michael S. Landy,et al.  Integration of stereopsis and motion shape cues , 1994, Vision Research.

[24]  P. Cavanagh,et al.  The contribution of color to motion parallax , 1995 .

[25]  M. Landy,et al.  Measurement and modeling of depth cue combination: in defense of weak fusion , 1995, Vision Research.

[26]  M. Landy,et al.  Discrimination of orientation-defined texture edges , 1995, Vision Research.

[27]  F. Kingdom,et al.  Sensitivity to orientation modulation in micropattern-based textures , 1995, Vision Research.

[28]  A. Yuille,et al.  Bayesian decision theory and psychophysics , 1996 .

[29]  Josée Rivest,et al.  Localizing contours defined by more than one attribute , 1996, Vision Research.

[30]  D. Regan,et al.  Vernier step acuity and bisection acuity for texture-defined form , 1997, Vision Research.

[31]  Michael S. Landy,et al.  Influence function for visual interpolation , 1997, Electronic Imaging.

[32]  J. Intriligator,et al.  Perceptual Learning of Orientation Discrimination by More Than One Attribute , 1997, Vision Research.

[33]  M. Landy,et al.  Long range interactions between oriented texture elements , 1999, Vision Research.

[34]  M. Landy,et al.  Interaction between the perceived shape of two objects , 1999, Vision Research.

[35]  L. Maloney Physics-based approaches to modeling surface color perception , 1999 .

[36]  M. Georgeson,et al.  Does early non-linearity account for second-order motion? , 1999, Vision Research.

[37]  S. Dakin Orientation variance as a quantifier of structure in texture. , 1999, Spatial vision.

[38]  Robert A. Jacobs,et al.  Modeling the Combination of Motion, Stereo, and Vergence Angle Cues to Visual Depth , 1999, Neural Computation.

[39]  David Whitaker,et al.  Localising Conflicting Visual Attributes , 2000 .

[40]  A. Mussap,et al.  Alignment of orientation-modulated textures , 2000, Vision Research.

[41]  Michael S. Landy,et al.  Motion-Disparity Interaction and the Scaling of Stereoscopic Disparity , 2001 .